簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 施宗翰
Tsung-Han Shih
論文名稱: 探究八年級學生對力持有的「意義」、「心智模式」與「內在一致性」
An investigation of 8th graders's "meanings of Force","mental models" and
指導教授: 邱美虹
Chiu, Mei-Hung
學位類別: 碩士
Master
系所名稱: 科學教育研究所
Graduate Institute of Science Education
論文出版年: 2008
畢業學年度: 96
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 131
中文關鍵詞: 迷思概念概念改變力的意義心智模式解釋一致性內在一致性
英文關鍵詞: misconception, conceptual change, meaning of force, mental model, explanatory consistence, internal consistence
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:157下載:14
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 摘要
    本研究是以Vosniadou(2002)的研究為基礎及使用相同研究的工具,並試圖探討學生在不同情境下持有「力的意義」,與「內在一致性」情形。研究對象為82名台北市八年級的學生,在無教學的情況下給予施測,並挑選30名學生作晤談,最後將這些資料編碼,作為量化與質性分析的來源。
    研究結果發現學生的心智模式不同於Vosniadou的結果,學生對於力的意義在不同情境下,會有改變的情形,不同於Vosniadou所宣稱:「近九成孩童的心智模式具有狹隘且內在一致性之力的解釋」。研究顯示學生的心智模式可分成四種模式,分別為「初始模式」、「漸進模式」、「混合模式」與「類科學模式」。本研究並無發現完全符合「科學模式」的類型。在同一個年齡層中,這四種模式有漸進的意涵,有如橫向的概念改變形式。本研究根據所定義的心智模式「內在一致性」,量化一致性的程度。研究結果顯示:四種心智模式中,以「類科學模式」的一致性最高,其次是「漸進模式」,接著是「混合模式」,最低為「初始模式」。
    最後,若能針對「混合模式」設計合適的教學,便能夠使學生的心智模式修正成為「類科學模式」或「科學模式」。

    Abstract
    The research was based on Vosniadou’s research(2002) using the same tool to investigate what meaning of force and internally consistent interpretation of force. 82 8th graders in Taipei city were tested without any instruction. Among these, 30 students were interviewed. Finally, coding these data for the source of quantitative and qualitative analysis were conducted.
    The finding of this study is different from Vosniadou’s. The students’ understanding of the meaning of the force changed in different contexts that is not similar as Vosniadou’s finding: most of the children(88.6%) made use of a small number of relatively well-defined and internally consistent interpretations of force.
    The results showed that the mental models of the students could be classified into four categories. These are” Initial Model”, “Progressive model”, “Mixed model” ,and “Similar scientific model”. The result showed that none of the students had scientific model. In the same age, these four models seem to be progressive gradually, as one kind of transversely conceptual change.The internal consistence was defined by the author and then is measured the degree of that in this study. The result showed that among these categories, the degree of consistence ” Similar scientific model” is superior, secondly ” Progressive model “, the next “Mixed model”, and the lowest is” Initial Model”.
    Finally, if we can design a suitable instruction in the light of “Mixed model”, the model held by the students can be revised to be “Similar scientific model” or “Scientific model”.

    第壹章 緒論 1 第一節 研究背景與動機 1 第二節 研究目的與問題 2 第三節 研究基本假設 3 第四節 名詞釋義 3 第五節 研究範圍與限制 4 第貳章 文獻探討 5 第一節 知識建構理論與迷思概念 5 第二節 概念改變的研究 11 第三節 心智模式的研究 16 第四節 Vosniadou的架構理論 19 第五節 diSessa的p-prim理論 23 第六節 力與運動的迷思概念 28 第參章 研究方法 32 第一節 研究對象 32 第二節 研究工具 32 第三節 研究流程 38 第四節 資料處理與分析 40 第肆章 研究結果與討論 43 第一節 學生持有力的意義 43 第二節 不同情境下學生對力的解釋情形 59 第三節 學生對力持有的心智模式 82 第四節 解釋一致性與內在一致性 91 第伍章 結論與建議 101 第一節 結論 101 第二節 建議 107 參考文獻 108 附錄一 力的概念測驗 114 附錄二 Vosniadou對於不同題組學生回答類型之分類表(表1至表9) 118

    中文文獻:
    全中平(民83)。師範學院學生對學習物理力學概念之分析研究。國立臺北師範學院學報,7, 481~506。
    全中平(民85)。國民小學五年級學生對學習力與運動概念之分析研究。國立臺北師範學院學報,9,405-426。
    余秀麗(2003)。探討國三學生對於重力概念之了解及心智模式。國立台灣師範大學碩士論文(未出版)。
    吳怡嫻(2006)。跨年級學生氣體心智模式演變歷程之探究與分析。國立台灣師範大學科學教育研究所碩士論文(未出版)。
    林靜雯(2000)。由概念改變及心智模式初探多重類比對國小四年級學生電學概念學習之影響。國立師範大學科學教育研究所碩士論文(未出版)。
    林靜雯(2005)。由概念演化觀點探究不同教科書教-學序列對不同心智模式學生電學學習之影響。國立台灣師範大學科學教育研究所博士論文(未出版)。
    邱美虹(1994)。從”自我解釋”所產生的推論探究高中生化學平衡的學習。師大學報,39,489-524。
    邱美虹(1996)。學習策略與科學學習。科學教育月刊,191,2-15。
    邱美虹(2000)。概念改變研究的省思與啟示。科學教育學刊,8(1),1-34。
    邱美虹等(譯)(2003)。P. Thagard著。概念革命(Conceptual Revolutions)。台北市:洪葉。
    郭重吉(民78)。利用晤談方式探查國中學生對重要物理概念的另有架構之研究(I)。中華民國第五屆科學教育學術研討會論文彙編,155-176。
    陳郡鳳(2005)。探討理想氣體動力論之建模教學對高一學生建構微觀氣體粒子運動心智模式的影響。國立台灣師範大學科學教育研究所碩士論文(未出版)。
    陳義勳(民85)。國小高年級學生自然科學中力學單元迷思概念之探討。臺北市立師範學院學報,27,83-104。
    彭泰源(1999)。國小五年級學童力與運動概念學習之研究。國立彰化師範大學科學教育研究所碩士論文(未出版)。
    曾舒平(2005)。探討高一學生對於「板塊構造運動」所持有之概念架構之解釋融貫性。國立台灣師範大學科學教育研究所碩士論文(未出版)。
    楊其安(1989)。利用臨床晤談探究國中學生對力學概念的另有架構。國立彰化師範大學科學教育研究所碩士論文(未出版)。
    董正玲(1991)。利用晤談方式探究國小兒童運動與力概念的另有架構。國立彰化師範大學科學教育研究所碩士論文(未出版)。
    劉俊庚(2002)。概念改變與概念改變教學策略之文獻分析-以概念圖和後社分析模式探討其意涵與影響。國立台灣師範大學科學教育研究所碩士論文(未出版)。
    鍾曉蘭(2006)。以多重表徵的模型教學探究高二學生理想氣體心智模式的類型及演變的途徑。國立台灣師範大學科學教育研究所碩士論文(未出版)。
     英文文獻:
    Abimbola, I. O. (1988). The problem of terminology in the study of student conceptions in science. Science Education, 72(2), 175-184.
    Bar, V. & Galili, l.(1994). Stages of childrens view about evaporation. Intemational Joumal of Science Education,16, 2,157-174.
    Berg, T., & Brouwer, W. (1991). Teacher awareness of student alternative conceptions about rotational motion and gravity. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28(1), 3-18.
    Bliss, J., Ogborn, J., & Whitelock, D. (1989). Secondary school pupils’ commonsense
    theories of motion. International Journal of Science Education, 11(3), 261-272.
    Bransford J D andVye N J (1989). A perspective on cognitive research and its implications for instruction' in Resnick L B and Klopfer L E (eds) Toward a thinking curriculum: current cognitive research (173-205) Alexandria VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.
    Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L.A. Hirschfeld and S.A. Gelman (Eds.) Mapping the mind (pp. 160-200). New York: Cambridge University Press.
    Chi, M. T. H. (1992). Conceptual change within and across ontological categories: example from learning and discovery in science. In R. Giere (Ed.), Cognitive models of science: Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science (pp.129-186). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
    Chi, M. T. H., deLeew, N., Chiu, M. H., & Lavancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding, Cognitive science, 18, 439-477
    Clement, J. (1993). Using bridging analogies and anchoring intuitions to deal with students’ preconceptions in physics. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 30(10), 1241-1257.
    diSessa, A. A. (1933). Toward a epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 105-225.
    diSessa, A. A. (1988). Knowledge in Pieces. Constructivism in the Computer Age. P. B. P. George Forman. Hillsdale, New Jersey, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. 1: 49-70.
    diSessa, A. A., Gillespie, N. M., & Esterly, J. B. (2004). Coherence versus fragmentation in the development of the concept of force. Cognitive Science, 28(6), 58.
    Driver, R. (1978) When is a stage not a stage? A critique of Piaget’s theory of cognitive development and its application to science education Educational Research, 21,(1), 54-61.
    Driver, R., & Easley, J. (1978). Pupils and paradigms: A review of literature related to concept development in adolescent science students. Studies in Education, 5, 61-84.
    Finley, F. (1985). Variations in prior knowledge. Science Education, 69(5), 697-705.
    Fischbein, E., Stavy, R., & Ma-Naim, H. (1989). The psychological structure of naïve impetus conceptions. International Journal of Science Education, 11(1), 71-81.
    Franco, C. & Colinvaux, D. (2000). Grasping mental models. In J. K. Gilbert & C. J. Boulter (Eds.), Developing Models in Science Education (pp. 93-118). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
    Gagne, R. M., (1985) The Conditions of Learning and Theory of Instruction. New York: CBS College Publishing.
    Gunstone, R. F., & White, R. T. (1986) Assessing understanding by means of Venn diagrams. Science Education, 70(2), 151-158.
    Johnson-Laird, P. (1987). The mental representation of the meaning of words. Cognition, 25, 189-211.
    Johnson-Laird, P. N.(1989). Mental models. In M.I Posner (Eds.),Foundations of Cognitive Science(pp.467-499).Cambridge, MA:MIT Press.
    Lewis, E., & Linn, M. (1994). Heat energy and temperature conceptions of adolescents, adults, and experts: Implications for curricular improvements. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 31(6), 657-677.
    McCloskey, M.(1983).Naive theories of motion.In D. Gentner& A. Stevens (Eds.), Mental models(pp.299-324).Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,Inc.
    Ministrell, J. (1982). Explaining the “at rest” condition of an object. The Physics Teacher, January, 10-14.
    Nersessian, N. J., & Resnick, L.B. (1989). Comparing historical and intuitive explanations of motion: Does “naive physics” have a structure? Proceedings of the 11th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 412-417). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Norman, D. A.(1983),Some observations on mental models. In D. Gentner and A. Stevens (Eds.), Mental Models(pp.7-14). Hillasdale, NJ:Erlbaum
    Pardhan H. & Bano Y.(2001). Science teachers' alternate conceptions about direct currents. International Journal of Science Education, 23[3], 301-318, 2001.
    Pella, M. O. (1966). Concept learning in science. Science Teacher, 33(9), 31-34.
    Piaget, J. (1972). The psychology of the child. New York: Basic Books.
    Piaget, J. (1990). The child's conception of the world. New York: Littlefield Adams.
    Posner, G. J, Strike, KA, Hewson, P W. (1982). Accomodation of a Scientific Conception: Toward a Theory of Conceptual Change. Science education. 66(2): 211–227
    Reif, F. & Allen, S. (1989). Interpreting and teaching scientific concepts: A study of acceleration. Cognition and Instruction. 9, 1-44.
    Taber, K. S. (1997). Student understanding of ionic bonding: molecular versus electrostatic
    framework? School Science Review, 78(285), 85-95.
    Thagard, P. (1992). Conceptual revolutions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
    Thijs, G. D. (1992). Evaluation of an introductory course on “force” considering students’ preconceptions. Science Education, 76(2), 155-174.
    Treagust, D. F. (1995). Diagnostic assessment of student’s science concepts. In S. Glynn & R. Duit (Eds.), Learning science in the schools: Research reforming practice (pp.327-346). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Trowbridge, J. E. & Mintzes, J. J. (1988). Alternative conceptions in animal classification: A cross-age study. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 25(7), 547-571.
    Trowbridge, J. E. & Mintzes, J. J.(1985). Students’ alternative conceptions of animals and animal classification. School science and mathematics, 85(4), 304-314.
    Trumper, R. (1997). A survey of conceptions of energy of Israeli pre-service high school
    biology teachers. International Journal of Science Education, 19(1), 31-46.
    von Glasersfeld,Ernst (1987) The Construction of Knowledge: Contributions to Conceptual Semantics.
    Vosniadou, S. (1989). On the Nature of Children's Naive Knowledge. Paper presented at the 11th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Hillsdale, NJ.
    Vosniadou, S. (1991). Conceptual development in astronomy. In B. Brotton (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning Science. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Vosniadou, S. (1991). Designing curricula for conceptual restructuring: Lesson for the study of knowledge acquisition in astronomy .Journal of curriculum studies , 23(3), 219-237.
    Vosniadou, S.,&Brewer, W. F.(1992). Mental models of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24(4), 535-585.
    Vosniadou, S. (1994). Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change [special issue]. Learning and instruction, 4, 45-69.
    Vosniadou, S. (2002). On the nature of naive physics. In M. Limon & L. Mason (Eds.), Reconsidering conceptual change: Issues in theory and practice (pp.61-76). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
    Vosniadou, S. and Ioannides, C. (1988). From conceptual development to science education: A psychological point of view. International Journal of Science Education, Vol., 20, 10, (pp.1213-1230).
    Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1987). Theories of knowledge restructuring in development. Review of Education Research, 57(1), 51-67.
    Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental meanings of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535-585.
    Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1994). Mental models of the day/night cycle.Cognitive Science, 18, 123-183.
    Vosniadou, S., & Ioannides, C. (1998). From conceptual development to science education: a psychological point of view. Intnational Journal of Science Education, 20(10), 1213-1230.
    Watts, D. M. (1983a). A study of schoolchildren’s alternative frameworks of the concept of force. European Journal of Science Education, 5(2), 217-230.
    White, B. (1983). Sources of difficulty in understanding Newtonian dynamics. Cognitive Science, 7(1), 41-65.
    White, B.Y. (1984). Designing computer games to help physics students understand Newton’s laws of motion. Cognition and instruction ,1(1),69-108.

    QR CODE