簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 黃書彥
Shu-Yen Huang
論文名稱: 台灣紫嘨鶇育雛期親鳥給食聲與警戒聲之研究
Feeding and alarm call of Formosa Whistling Thrush (Myiophoneus insularis) during nestling period
指導教授: 王穎
Wang, Ying
學位類別: 碩士
Master
系所名稱: 生命科學系
Department of Life Science
論文出版年: 2007
畢業學年度: 95
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 45
中文關鍵詞: 台灣紫嘯鶇給食聲警戒聲育雛護巢
英文關鍵詞: Myiophoneus insularis, feeding call, alarm call, breeding behavior, nest defence
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:140下載:27
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 本研究於2005年針對台灣紫嘯鶇(Myiophoneus insularis)親鳥育雛期間發出的給食聲與警戒聲進行研究。在石碇樣區地共觀察了10對親鳥,並於雛鳥0-11日齡進行錄影,共記錄237小時,1267趟餵食。親鳥餵食前發出給食聲的頻度在雛鳥不同日齡間有顯著差異(P<0.0001):於雛鳥0-1日齡(73.04±29.35%)、2-3日齡(76.71±20.48%)、4-5日齡(67.49±26.88%),及6-7日齡(58.47±24.18%)之間無顯著差異,但顯著高於8-9日齡(23.24±12.15%)及10-11日齡(11.74±12.18%)。就親鳥性別而言,雄鳥發出給食聲頻度(53.36±30.29 %)大於雌鳥(41.73±33.17 %),但兩者未有顯著差異(P=0.0761)。就背景聲音對給食聲的影響而言,築於橋墩的巢(66±4.3 dB, n=5)大於築於房舍的巢(<50 dB ,n=5),較大的背景聲會影響雛鳥察覺親鳥回巢的能力。於雛鳥8-11日齡,築巢於橋墩的親鳥發出給食聲頻度為22.45±4.55%,顯著大於築巢於房舍者的10.84±4.79 %(P=0.009)。此外親鳥發出給食聲的下一趟餵食間隔為1140±525 秒,顯著長於未發出者的873±349 秒(P=0.0048),且帶回食物大於1嘴喙長的機會也較大,然未有顯著差異(P =0.0795)。以親鳥給食聲、溝通聲,以及藍鵲叫聲進行對本種雛鳥的回播實驗結果有顯著差異(P<0.0001)。雛鳥對於親鳥給食聲的乞食張嘴時間及乞食強度皆顯著大於親鳥溝通聲及藍鵲叫聲,因此本種雛鳥應具有正確辨別給食聲的能力。本種警戒聲可區分為三型:警戒聲Ⅰ-a音頻在4758-7040 Hz之間,主頻率為5930 Hz;警戒聲Ⅰ-b音頻在3239-6630 Hz之間,主頻率為5737 Hz;警戒聲Ⅱ音頻在2408-4851 Hz之間,主頻率為3668 Hz。親鳥於護巢時發出的警戒聲總次數,於雛鳥不同階段間有顯著差異(P=0.002)。於雛鳥早期,親鳥警戒聲次數較少,以高頻且頻域窄的為警戒聲Ⅰ-a為主;而雛鳥中期以後,親鳥警戒聲次數增多,以較低頻且頻域寬的警戒聲Ⅰ-b為主。而聲音最為粗啞的警戒聲Ⅱ只在雛鳥中期之後才會使用,並伴隨較強烈的護巢行為。以親鳥警戒聲Ⅰ-a、Ⅰ-b、Ⅱ進行對本種雛鳥的回播實驗結果顯示,雛鳥聽到回播後動作次數顯著較回播前低,表示三種聲音皆具有警告雛鳥的功能。

    The parental feeding and the alarm call of Taiwan whistling thrush (Myiophoneus insularis)were investigated at Shrding Hsiang. The parental feeding call was studied via video recorder while the nestlings were 0-11 days old. Observations were conducted on 10 nests, total 162 hours and 1,525 feeding times. The frequency of the parental feeding was different between the nestling ages(P<0.0001). Both male and female called proportionally more at the early stage (0-1, 2-3, 4-5, 6-7 days old), and then significantly decreased when they were 8-9, and 10-11 days old. Male called rather more than female, but not significantly(P=0.0761). Parents spent significantly longer away from the nest immediately after uttering feeding call(P=0.0048),and tend to bring larger food(>1 bit). The background noise hindered the nestlings’ detection while parents arrived the nest. The background noise around the bridge(66±4.3 dB, n=5)was higher than the house(<50 dB ,n=5). Parents which nested in the bridge called more feeding call than in the house at the nestling’ 8-11 days old (P=0.009). The playback experiment revealed that nestlings begged significantly longer time and higher intensity to the parental feeding call than the parental social call and Urocissa caerulea call(begging time, P<0.0001; begging intensity P<0.0001), it might indicate that nestlings could discriminate the feeding call accurately. I recorded three types of the alarm call during parents nest defense. The frequency of the alarm callⅠ-a was between 4,758 to 7,040 Hz, and the average of the dominant frequency is 5,930 Hz. The frequency of the alarm callⅠ-b is between 3,239 to 6,630 Hz, and the average of the dominant frequency is 5,737 Hz. The frequency of the alarm call Ⅱ is between 2,408 to 4,851 Hz, and the average of the dominant frequency is 3,668 Hz. The number of the alarm call, per 3 min during parents nest defense, was different between the nestling stages(P<0.002). Parents called less at nestlings’ early stage(0-5 days old), and the type of the call was mainlyⅠ-a (highest frequency whistles call). At middle (6- 11days old)and late stage(12-17 days old), parents significantly called more, and the type of call was mainly Ⅰ-b (broadest frequency range call). Furthermore, the type Ⅱ(lowest frequency call)alarm call was recorded only at middle and late stage, and always accompanied with stronger nest defense. In the playback experiment, it revealed that nestlings moved much more less while playing back the alarm call (Ⅰ-a, P=0.018; Ⅰ-b, P=0.017; Ⅱ, P=0.017), it can be explained that all the three alarm calls are warning sounds; however, I don’t know about the function of each alarm call enough to be able to describe the details.

    摘要 I 前言 1 研究對象 5 研究樣區 6 研究方法 7 資料分析 11 結果 12 討論 18 參考文獻 26 圖目 31 表目 43

    Alcock, J. 2005. Animal Behaviour: An Evolutionary Approach, 8thed. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
    Andersson, M., Wiklund, C. G. & Rundgren, H. 1980. Parental defence of offspring: a model and an example. Anim. Behav. 28:536-542.
    Bachman, G. C. & Chappell, M.A. 1998. The energetic cost of begging behaviour in nestling house wrens. Anim. Behav. 55:1607-1618.
    Barash, D. P. 1975. Evolutionary aspects of parental behavior of Alpine accentor. Wilson Bull 87:367-373.
    Bengtsson, H. & Rydén, O. 1981. Development of parent-young interaction in asynchronously hatched broods of Altricial birds. Z. Tierpsychol. 56:255-272.
    Boucher, D. H. 1977. On wasting parental investment. Am. Nat., 111(980):786-788.
    Catchpole, C. K. & Slater, P. J. B. 1995. Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge University Press.
    Clemmons, J. R. 1995. Vocalizations and other stimuli that elicit gaping in nestling black-capped chickadees. Auk 112(3):603-612.
    Coleman, R. M. & Gross, M. R. 1991. Parental Investment Theory :The Role of Past Investment. Tree 12:404-406.
    Davies, N. B., Madden, J. R. & Butchart, S. H. M. 2004. Learning fine-tunes a specific response of nestlings to the parental alarm calls of their own species. Proc. R. Soc. B 271:297-2304.
    Dearborn, D.C. 1998. Begging behavior and food acquisition by brown-headed cowbird nestling. Behav. Eco. Sociobiol. 43:448-457.
    Dyck, M. G. & Baydack, R. K. 2004. Vigilance behaviour of polar bear (Ursus maritimus) in the context of wildlife-viewing activities at Churchill, Manitoba, Canada. Biol. Cons. 116:343-350.
    Gill, S.A. & Sealy, G. S. 2003. Tests of two functions of alarm calls given by yellow warblers during nest defence. Can. J. Zool., 81:1685-1690.
    Greig-Smith, P. W. 1980. Parental investment in nest defence by stonechays (Saxicola Torquata). Anim. Behav., 28:604-619.
    Grieco, F. 2001. Short-term regulation of food-provisioning rate and effect on prey size in blue tits, Parus caeruleus. Anim. Behavi. 62:107-116.
    Grieco, F. 2002. Time constraint on food choice in provisioning blue tit (Patus caeruleus):the relationship between feeding rate and prey size. Behavi. 64:517-526.
    Halupka, K. 1998. Vocal begging by nestlings and vulnerability to nest predation in meadow pipits Anthus pratensis: to what extent do predation cost of begging exist? Ibis 140:144-149.
    Harvey, P. H. & Greenwood, P. J. 1978. Antipredator defence strategies: some evolutionary problems. Pp. 129-151 in Behavioural ecology (J. K. Krebs & N. B. Davies, Eds). Sunderland, Massachusetts, Sinauer Assoc.
    Hofstetter, S. H. & Ritchison, G.. 1998. The begging behavior of nestling eastern screech-owls. Wilson Bull 10:86-92..
    Kilner, R. M., Noble, D. G. & Davies, N. B. 1999. Signals of need in parent-offspring communication and their exploitation by the common cuckoo. Nature 397:667-672.
    Kilner, R. M. 2001. A growth cost of begging in captive canary chicks. Proceeding of National Academy of Science, U.S.A., 98:11394-11398.
    Knight, R. L. & Temple, S. A. 1986. Why does intensity of avian nest defense increase during the nestling cycle? Auk 103:318-327.
    Krebs, H., Weyers, P., Macht, M., Weijers, H. G. & Janke, W. 1997. Scanning behaviour of rats during eating under stressful noise. Physiol. Behav. 62:151-154.
    Leech, S. M. & Leonard. 1996. Is there energetic cost to begging tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor)? Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263:983-987.
    Leech, S. M. & Leonard, M. L. 1997. Begging and the risk of predation in nestling bird. Behav. Ecol. 8:644-646.
    Leonard, M. L & Horn, A. G. 1996. Provisioning rules in tree swallows. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 38:341-347.
    Leonard, M. L., Fernandez, N. & Brown, G. 1997. Parental calls and nestling behaviour in tree swallows. Auk 114(4):668-672.
    Leonard, M. L & Horn, A. G. 1998. Need and nestmates affect begging in tree swallows. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42:431-436.
    Leonard, M. L.& Horn, A. G. 2001. Begging in absence of parents by nestling tree swallows. Behav. Ecol. 12(4):501-505.
    Leonard, M. L., Horn, A. G. & Mukhida, A. 2005. False alarms and begging in nesting birds. Anim. Behavi. 69:701-708.
    Madden, J. R., Kilner, R. M. & Davies, N. B. 2005. Nestling responses to adult food and alarm calls: 1. Species-specific responses in two cowbird hosts. Anim. Behav. 70:619-627.
    Marler, P. 1955. Characteristics of some animal call. Nature. 176:6-8.
    Maurer, G., Magrath, R. D., Leonard, M.L., Horn, A. G. & Donnelly, C. 2003. Begging to differ: scrubwren nestlings beg to alarm calls and vocalize when parents are absent. Anim. Behav. 65:1045-1055.
    Moreno-Rueda, G. 2005. A trade-off between predation risk and sibling competition in the begging behavior of Coal and Great Tits. J. Field Ornithol. 76(4):390-394.
    Morton, E. S. 1977. On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird mammal sounds. Am. Nat. 111,855-869.
    Morton, E. S. 1982. Grading, discreteness, redundancy, and motivation-structural rules. In Acoustic Communication in Birds, Vol. 1, pp.183-212. Academic Press, New York.
    Montgomerie, R. D. & Weatherhead. 1988. Risks and rewards of nest defence by parent birds. The Quarterly Review of Biology 63(2):167-187.
    Nuechterlein, G. L. 1988. Parent-Young Vocal Communication in Western Grebes. Condor 90:632-636.
    Pavel, V & Bures, S. 2001. Offspring age and nest defence: test of the feedback hypothesis in the pipit. Anim. Behav. 61:297-303.
    Platzen, D., & Magrath, R. D. 2004. Parental alarm calls suppress nestling vocalization. Proc. R. Soc. B 271:1271-1276.
    Platzen, D., & Magrath, R. D. 2005. Adaptive difference in response to two types of parental alarm call in altricial nestlings. Proc. R. Soc. B 272:1101-1106.
    Quinn, J. L., Whittingham, M. J., Butler, S. J. & Cresswel, W. 2006. Noise, predation risk compensation and vigilance in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. J. Avian Biol. 37:601-608.
    Redondo, T & Carranza, J. 1989. Offspring reproductive value and nest defence in the magpie (Pica pica). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 25:369-378.
    Roulin, A. 2001. On the cost of begging vocalization: implications of vigilance. Behav. Ecol. 12(4):506-515.
    Rodrıíguez-Gironés, M. A., Zúñiga, J. M. & Redondo, T. 2001. Effect of begging on growth rates of nestling chicks. Behav. Ecol. 12(3):269-274.
    RytkÖnen, P., Orell, M. & Koivula. 1995. Pseudo Concorde fallacy in the willow tit?. Anim. Behav. 49:1017-1028.
    Sacchi, R., Saino, N. & Galeotti, P. 2002. Features of begging calls reveal general condition and need of food of barn swallow (Hirundo rustica) nestlings. Behav. Ecol. 13(2):268-273.
    Trivers, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. Pp. 139-179 in sexual selection and the descent of man (B. Campbell, Ed.). Chicago, Aldine.
    Winkler, w. d. 1987. A general model for parental care. Am. Nat. 140(4):526-543.
    方志仁。2004。台灣紫嘯鶇的領域與鳴叫行為。碩士論文。國立台灣師範大學。
    王嘉雄,吳森雄,黃光瀛,楊秀英,蔡仲晃,蔡牧起,蕭慶亮。1991。台灣野鳥圖鑑。亞舍圖書有限公司。
    王穎,裴家騏。1984。紫嘯鶇育雛行為之初步研究。師大生物學報19:83-92。
    王穎。1984。台灣特有種紫嘯鶇的生態研究。國家科學委員會研究報告。
    張萬福。1997。台灣的陸鳥。中台科學技術出版社。153頁。
    莊美真。2006。青背山雀雛鳥乞食行為之探討。碩士論文。國立台灣師範大學。
    黃永貴。2001。影響台灣燕雀目鳥類名唱聲音結構之因素。碩士論文。國立台灣大學。
    蔡航椰。1980。台灣紫嘯鶇築巢位置與育雛行為。野鳥1:62-65。
    顏重威。1994。台灣的野生鳥類(一)留鳥。渡假出版社。71-72頁。

    QR CODE