簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 李維光
Lee, Wei-Kuang
論文名稱: 捷思歷程中的反應強度評估與衝突評估之驗證
Verifying the response intensity evaluation and conflict evaluation in the heuristic process
指導教授: 吳昭容
Wu, Chao-Jung
學位類別: 博士
Doctor
系所名稱: 教育心理與輔導學系
Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling
論文出版年: 2019
畢業學年度: 107
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 109
中文關鍵詞: 二元評估模式分析介入三階段模式基礎率忽略問題蒙提霍爾問題雙系統理論邏輯直覺模式
英文關鍵詞: Base-rate neglect problem, Dual evaluation model, Dual process theory, Logical intuition model, Monty Hall problem, Three-stage dual-process model of analytic engagement
DOI URL: http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU201900292
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:142下載:18
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 本研究旨在建構二元評估雙系統理論模式,以描述個體在面對推理問題時的思考歷程,並以基礎率忽略題及蒙提霍爾題作為實驗作業,探究二元評估模式中,Type 1歷程「反應強度評估」以及「衝突評估」這兩個後設認知的運作機制。研究結果顯示:一、在「衝突評估」方面,不管在快速回應或是完整敘述版本的基礎率忽略題,受試者在衝突題都呈現反應時間較長、答題信心較低的結果。但在蒙提霍爾題,僅有高邏輯組受試者的衝突題答題信心及中獎機率預估選擇信心較非衝突題低,低邏輯組則無顯著差異。即便增加蒙提霍爾題的困難度,受試者仍敏感於初始反應間的衝突性。二、在「反應強度評估」方面,受試者面對快速回應版本基礎率忽略題,高邏輯組非衝突題答題信心及認知脫鉤反應答題信心都高於低邏輯組,但合理化反應答題信心沒有較低、反應時間也沒有較長。面對完整敘述版的題目時,高邏輯組非衝突題答題信心較高、反應時間也較長,高邏輯組合理化反應時間也較低邏輯組長,但認知脫鉤反應和合理化反應的答題信心都沒有符合研究預期。至於蒙提霍爾題,高邏輯組非衝突題答題信心、中獎機率預估選擇信心都高於低邏輯組,但認知脫鉤反應和合理化反應的答題信心也都沒有符合研究預期。
    整體來說,本研究有四個主要發現,第一、證實二元評估模式Type 1歷程中,有反應強度評估及衝突評估兩種後設認知的觀點,且反應強度評估啟動先於衝突評估。第二,在衝突性及強度評估方面,受試者在基礎率忽略題的敏感性都較MHP好。第三,不管是基礎率忽略題還是MHP,受試者對於非衝突題強度評估敏感度較衝突題佳。第四,在衝突敏感度上,受試者對於基礎率忽略題,不論高低邏輯都能展現出衝突敏感度,但對於MHP,受試者僅對高邏輯題目有衝突敏感度。

    This study aims to construct a dual evaluation model to describe the individual's thinking process in the face of reasoning problems. We use the base-rate neglect problem and the Monty Hall problem as tasks, to explore the two mechanisms of metacognitive process: “response intensity evaluation” and “conflict evaluation”. Results showed that, First, in the aspect of "conflict evaluation", regardless of the rapid-response or the full narrative version, the conflict group showed a longer reaction time and lower confidence than the congruent group. However, in MHP, only the high logic group subjects' response confidence and probability choice confidence of the conflict items were lower than the congruent items, while the low logic group had no significant difference. Subjects were still sensitive to the conflict between the initial responses even in the case of difficulty improvement. Second, in the "response intensity evaluation", high logic group showed lower response confidence of congruent items and higher response confidence of decoupling response in the rapid-response version of the base-rate neglect problem. In the full narrative version, high logic group showed higher response confidence, long response time and long rationalization response time of congruent items. As for the MHP, the response confidence and probability choice confidence of the high logic group on congruent items were higher than the low logic group. However, there were no significant difference in the confidence of the high and low logic groups in rationalization and cognitive decoupling response.
    Overall, there were four main findings in this study. First, we confirmed that in the Type 1 process of the dual evaluation model, there are two kinds of meta-cognition, “response intensity evaluation” and “conflict evaluation”, and the response intensity evaluation starts before the conflict evaluation. Second, the subjects of base-rate neglect problem showed better sensitivity of the conflict and response intensity than those of MHP. Third, the subjects showed higher response intensity sensitivity to congruent items than the conflict ones. Fourth, Subjects had conflict sensitivity for base-rate neglect problem, regardless of high or low logic groups, but for MHP, only high logic groups had conflict sensitivity.

    誌謝 i 中文摘要 iii 英文摘要 v 目次 vii 表次 ix 圖次 xi 緒論 1 雙系統理論簡介 2 傳統雙系統理論探討雙系統的互動與時間序 3 雙驅模式的雙系統理論 9 雙系統理論與蒙提霍爾問題 15 研究理念與假設 18 實驗一 29 研究方法 30 研究結果 33 討論 37 實驗二 41 研究方法 42 研究結果 43 討論 47 實驗三 51 研究方法 51 研究結果 54 討論 59 實驗四 61 研究方法 62 研究結果 63 討論 67 綜合討論 71 本研究主要發現 71 本研究貢獻與限制 75 待探究議題及未來研究建議 78 參考文獻 85 中文部分 85 西文部分 85 附錄 99 附錄一 預選團體與特質列表 99 附錄二 特質團體配對問卷舉隅 100 附錄三 實驗一32版實驗程式訊息舉隅 102 附錄四 實驗一答題信心評估表 106 附錄五 單純主要效果摘要表 107 附錄六 實驗三蒙提霍爾實驗施測題目舉隅 109

    楊國樞(1999):中國人之基本性格向度、結構及效應的系統性研究。國科會特約研究計畫成果報告 (NSC86-2143-H002-026)。台北:行政院國家科學委員會。
    Bago, B., & De Neys, W. (2017). Fast logic? Examining the time course assumption of dual process theory. Cognition, 158, 90–109. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.014
    Bago, B., & De Neys, W. (2019a). Advancing the specification of dual process models of higher cognition: a critical test of the hybrid model view. Thinking & Reasoning, published online. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2018.1552194
    Bago, B., & De Neys, W. (2019b). The Smart System 1: evidence for the intuitive nature of correct responding on the bat-and-ball problem, Thinking & Reasoning, 25(3), 257-299, doi: 10.1080/13546783.2018.1507949
    Bago, B., Raoelison, M., & De Neys, W. (2019). Second-guess: Testing the specificity of error detection in the bat-and-ball problem. Acta Psychologica, 193, 214-228. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.01.008
    Ball, L. J., Philips, P., Wade, C. N., & Quayle, J. D. (2006). Effects of belief and logic on syllogistic reasoning: Eye-movement evidence for selective processing models. Experimental Psychology, 53, 77–86. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.77
    Ball, L., Thompson, V., & Stupple, E. (2017). Conflict and dual process theory: the case of belief bias. In W. De Neys (Ed.), Dual Process Theory 2.0. (pp. 100-120). Oxon, UK: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9781315204550-7
    Bar-Hillel, M. (1989). Discussion: How to solve probability teasers. Philosophy of Science, 56, 348–358. doi: 10.1086/289493
    Bonner, C., & Newell, B. R. (2010). In conflict with ourselves? An investigation of heuristic and analytic processes in decision making. Memory and Cognition, 38(2), 186–196. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.2.186
    Brisson, J., Schaeken, W., Markovits, H., & De Neys, W. (2018). Conflict detection and logical complexity. Psychologica Belgica, 58, 318-333. doi: 10.5334/pb.448
    Burns, B. D., & Wieth, M. (2004). The collider principle in causal reasoning: Why the Monty Hall dilemma is so hard. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 434–449. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.434
    Carpenter, T. P., Corbitt, M. K., Kepner, H. S., Lindquist, M. M., & Reys, R. (1981). Results from the second mathematics assessment of the National Assessment of Educational Progress. Washington, DC: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
    De Neys, W. (2006). Automatic-heuristic and executive-analytic processing during reasoning: Chronometric and dualtask considerations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1070–1100. doi: 10.1080/02724980543000123
    De Neys, W. (2012). Bias and conflict: A case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 28–38. doi: 10.1177/1745691611429354
    De Neys, W. (2014). Conflict detection, dual processes, and logical intuitions: Someclarifications. Thinking and Reasoning, 20(2), 169–187. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2013.854725
    De Neys, W. (2015). Heuristic bias and conflict detection during thinking. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 62, 1–32. doi: 10.1016/bs.plm.2014.09.001
    De Neys, W. (2017). Bias, conflict, and fast logic: Towards a hybrid dual process future? In W. De Neys (Ed.), Dual Process Theory 2.0. Oxon, UK: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9781315204550-4
    De Neys, W., & Feremans, V. (2013). Development of heuristic bias detection in elementaryschool. Developmental Psychology, 49, 258–69. doi: 10.1037/a0028320
    De Neys, W., & Franssens, S. (2009). Belief inhibition during thinking: Not always winning but at least taking part. Cognition, 113, 45–61. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.009
    De Neys, W., & Glumicic, T. (2008). Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking. Cognition, 106, 1248–1299. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.002
    De Neys, W., Moyens, E., & Vansteenwegen, D. (2010). Feeling we’re biased: Autonomic arousal and reasoning conflict. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 208–216. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.2.208
    De Neys, W., Novitskiy, N., Ramautar, J., & Wagemans, J. (2010). What makes a good reasoner?: Brain potentials and heuristic bias susceptibility. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 32, 1020 – 1025.
    De Neys, W., & Pennycook, G. (in press). Logic, fast and slow: Advances in dual-process theorizing. Current Directions in Psychological Science. doi: 10.1177/0963721415604610
    De Neys, W., Rossi, S., & Houdé, O. (2013). Bats, balls, and substitution sensitivity: Cognitive misers are no happy fools. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(2), 269–273. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0384-5
    De Neys, W., Vartanian, O., & Goel, V. (2008). Smarter than we think: When our brains detect that we are biased. Psychological Science, 19, 483 – 489. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02113.x
    De Neys, W., & Verschueren, N. (2006). Working memory capacity and a notorious brain teaser: The case of the Monty Hall dilemma. Experimental Psychology, 53, 123–131. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.123
    DeWolf, M., Grounds, M. A., Bassok, M., & Holyoak, K. J. (2014). Magnitude comparison with different types of rational numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(1), 71-82 doi: 10.1037/a0032916
    Dujmović, M. & Valerjev, P. (2018). The influence of conflict monitoring on meta-reasoning and response times in a base rate task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. OnlineFirst (Free Access until March 31st). 1–14. 10.1177/1747021817746924. doi: 10.1177/1747021817746924
    Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologists, 49, 709–724. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.709
    Evans, J. S. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press.
    Evans, J. S. B. T. (2002). Logical and human reasoning: An assessment of the deductive paradigm. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 978–996. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.128.6.978
    Evans, J. S. B. T. (2006). The heuristic‐analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13(3), 378–395. doi: 10.3758/BF03193858
    Evans, J. St. B. T. (2007). On the resolution of conflict in dual process theories. Thinking & Reasoning, 13, 321–339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780601008825
    Evans, J. St. B. T., & Curtis-Holmes, J. (2005). Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 11, 382–389. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546780542000005
    Evans, J. S. B. T, & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223–241. doi: 10.1177/1745691612460685
    Franco-Watkins, A. M., Derks, P. L., & Dougherty, M. R. P. (2003). Reasoning in the Monty Hall problem: Examining choice behaviour and probability judgements. Thinking and Reasoning, 9, 67–90 doi: 10.1080/13546780244000114
    Franssens, S., & De Neys, W. (2009). The effortless nature of conflict detection during thinking. Thinking and Reasoning, 15, 105–128. doi: 10.1080/13546780802711185
    Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25–42. doi: 10.1257/089533005775196732
    Frey D., Bago, B., & De Neys, W. (2017). Commentary: Seeing the conflict: an attentional account of reasoning errors. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1284. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01284
    Frey, D., Johnson, E.D., & De Neys, W. (2017). Individual differences in conflict detection during reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2017.1313283.
    Friedman, D. (1998). Monty Hall’s three doors: Construction and deconstruction of a choice Anomaly, American Economic Review, 88, 933–946. Retrieved from: http://www.jstor.org/stable/pdfplus/117012.
    Gigerenzer, G. (1991). How to make cognitive illusions disappear: Beyond “heuristics and biases”. European Review of Social Psychology, 2, 83–115. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779143000033.
    Granberg, D. (1999). Cross-cultural comparison of responses to the Monty Hall dilemma. Social Behavior and Personality, 27, 431–438. doi: 10.2224/sbp.1999.27.4.431
    Granberg, D., & Brown, T. A. (1995). The Monty Hall dilemma. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 711–723. doi: 10.1177/0146167295217006
    Granberg, D., & Dorr, N. (1998). Further exploration of two-stage decision making in the Monty Hall dilemma. American Journal of Psychology, 111, 561–579. doi: 10.2307/1423551
    Handley, S. J., Newstead, S. E., & Trippas, D. (2011). Logic, beliefs, and instruction: A test of the default interventionist account of belief bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(1), 28–43. doi: 10.1037/a0021098
    Handley, S. J., & Trippas, D. (2015). Dual processes and the interplaybetween knowledge and structure: A new parallel processing model. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 62, 33–58. doi: 10.1016/bs.plm.2014.09.002
    Herbranson, W. T., & Schroeder, J. (2010). Are birds smarter than mathematicians? Pigeons (Columba livia) perform optimally on a version of the Monty Hall dilemma. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 124, 1–13. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017703
    Hinson, J.M., Jameson, T.L., & Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 298–306. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.298
    Hirao, T., Murphy, T. I., & Masaki, H. (2016). Stimulus-preceding negativity represents a conservative response tendency. NeuroReport, 27, 80–84. doi: https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0000000000000495.
    Hirao, T., Murphy, T. I., & Masaki, H. (2017). Brain activities associated with learning of the Monty Hall dilemma task. Psychophysiology, 54, 1359–1369. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12883
    Howard, J. N., Lambdin, C. G., & Datteri, D. L. (2007). Let’s make a deal: Quality and avail ability of second-stage information as a catalyst for change. Thinking and Reasoning, 13, 248–272. doi: 10.1080/13546780600848049
    Johnson, E. D., Tubau, E., & De Neys, W. (2016). The Doubting System 1: Evidence for automatic substitution sensitivity. Acta Psychologica, 164, 56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.12.008
    Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2013.766389
    Kahneman, D., & Amos Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291. doi: 10.2307/1914185
    Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2005). A Model of Heuristic Judgment. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 267-293). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.
    Keren, G., & Schul, Y. (2009). Two is not always better than one: A critical evaluation of two-system theories. Perspectives on psychological science, 4, 533–550. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01164
    Klein, E. D., Evans, T. A., Schultz, N. B., & Beran, M. J. (2013). Learning how to “make a deal”: Human (Homo sapiens) and monkey (Macaca mulatta) performance when repeatedly faced with the Monty Hall dilemma. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127, 103 -108.doi:10.1037/a0029057
    Kramer, T., & Block, L. (2008). Conscious and non-conscious components of superstitious beliefs in judgment and decision-making. The Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 783–793. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/523288
    Krauss, S., & Wang, X. T. (2003). The psychology of the Monty Hall Problem: Discovering psychological mechanisms for solving a tenacious brain teaser. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 3–22. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.3
    Kruglanski, A. W., & Gigerenzer, G. (2011). Intuitive and deliberative judgements are based on common principles. Psychological Review, 118, 97–109. doi: 10.1037/a0020762
    Lubin, A., Houdé, O., & De Neys, W. (2015). Evidence for children's error sensitivity during arithmetic word problem solving. Learning and Instruction, 40, 1-8. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.07.005
    Mata, A., Ferreira, M. B., Voss, A., and Kollei, T. (2017). Seeing the conflict: an attentional account of reasoning errors. Psychon. Bul. Rev. 24, 1–7.doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1234-7
    Mazur, J. E., & Kahlbaugh, P. E. (2012). Choice behavior of pigeons (Columba livia), college students, and preschool children (Homo sapiens) in the Monty Hall dilemma. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 407-420. doi:10.1037/a0028273
    Mevel, K., Poirel, N., Rossi, S., Cassotti, M., Simon, G., Houdé, O., & De Neys, W. (2015). Bias detection: Response confidence evidence for conflict sensitivity in the ratio bias task. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(2), 227–237. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.986487
    Newman, I., Gibb, M., & Thompson, V. A. (2017). Rule-based easoning is fast and belief-based reasoning can be slow: Challenging current explanations of belief bias and base-rate neglect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 43(7), 1154–1170. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000372.supp
    Osman, M. (2004). An evaluation of dual-process theories of reasoning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 988–1010. doi: 10.3758/BF03196730
    Padberg, F. (2009). Didaktik der Bruchrechnung (4th ed.). Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-46184-6_3
    Page, S.E. (1998). Let’s make a deal. Economics Letters, 61, 175–180. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1765(98)00158-X
    Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2014). Cognitive style and religiosity: The role of conflict detection. Memory and Cognition, 42(1), 1–10. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0340-7
    Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A., & Koehler, D. J. (2012). Are we good at detecting conflict during reasoning? Cognition, 124(1), 101–106. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.004
    Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A., & Koehler, D. J. (2015). What makes us think? A three-stage dual-process model of analytic engagement. Cognitive Psychology, 80, 34–72. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.05.001
    Pennycook, G., & Thompson, V. A. (2012). Reasoning with base rates is routine, relatively effortless, and context dependent. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 528–534. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-0120249-3
    Pennycook, G., Trippas, D., Handley, S. J., & Thompson, V. A. (2014). Base rates: Both neglected and intuitive. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 40(2), 544–554. doi: 10.1037/a0034887.supp
    Petrocelli, J. V., & Harris, A. K. (2011). Learning inhibition in the Monty Hall Problem: The role of dysfunctional counterfactual prescriptions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1297–1311. doi: 10.1177/0146167211410245
    Risen, J. L. (2016). Believing what we do not believe: Acquiescence to superstitious beliefs and other powerful intuitions. Psychological Review, 123(2), 182–207. doi: 10.1037/rev0000017
    Risen, J. L., & Gilovich, T. (2008). Why people are reluctant to tempt fate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 293–307. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.2.293
    Rossi, S., Cassotti, M., Agogu e, M., & De Neys, W. (2013). Development of substitution bias sensitivity: Are adolescents happy fools? Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 35, 3321–3326.
    Saenen, L., Heyvaert, M., Van Dooren, W., & Onghena, P. (2018). Why humans fail in solving the Monty Hall dilemma: A systematic review. Psychologica Belgica. 58(1), 128–158. doi:http://doi.org/10.5334/pb.274
    Saenen, L., Van Dooren, W., & Onghena, P. (2015) A randomised Monty Hall experiment: The positive effect of conditional frequency feedback, Thinking and Reasoning, 21, 176-192, doi:10.1080/13546783.2014.918562
    Shafir, E., Smith, E. E., & Osherson, D. (1990). Typicality and reasoning fallacies. Memory & Cognition, 18, 229-239. doi: 10.3758/BF03213877
    Slembeck, T., & Tyran, J.-R. (2004). Do institutions promote rationality? An experimental study of the three-door anomaly. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 54, 337-350. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2003.03.002
    Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.3
    Sloman, S. A. (2002). Two systems of reasoning. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 379 – 398).Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511808098.024
    Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Stanovich, K. E. (2009). Is it time for a tri-process theory? Distinguishing the reflective and algorithmic mind. In J. St. B. T. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp. 55–88). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199230167.003.0003
    Stanovich, K. E. (2018). Miserliness in human cognition: the interaction of detection, override and mindware. Thinking and Reasoning, 24(4), 423-444, doi:10.1080/13546783.2018.1459314
    Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–726. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00003435
    Stibel, J. M., Dror, I. E., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2009). The collapsing choice theory: Dissociating choice and judgment in decision making. Theory and Decision, 66, 149–179. doi:10.1007/s11238-007-9094-7
    Stupple, E. J., Ball, L. J., & Ellis, D. (2013). Matching bias in syllogistic reasoning: Evidence for a dual-process account from response times and confidence ratings. Thinking and Reasoning, 19(1), 54–77. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2012.735622
    Szaszi, B., Palfi, B., Szollosi, A., Kieslich, P. J., & Aczel, B. (2018). Thinking dynamics and individual differences: Mouse-tracking analysis of the denominator neglect task. Judgment and Decision Making, 13(1), 23–32.
    Thompson, V.A., & Johnson, S.C. (2014). Conflict, metacognition, and analytic thinking. Thinking and Reasoning, 20(2), 215–244. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2013.869763
    Thompson, V. A., & Morsanyi, K. (2012). Analytic thinking: Do you feel like it? Mind and Society, 11, 93–105. doi: 10.1007/s11299-012-0100-6
    Thompson, V. A., Pennycook, G., Trippas, D., & Evans, J. S. B. T. (2018). Do smart people have better intuitions? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(7), 945-961.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000457
    Thompson, V. A., Turner, J. A. P., & Pennycook, G. (2011). Intuition, reason, and metacognition. Cognitive Psychology, 63, 107–140. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.06.001
    Travers, E., Rolison, J. J., & Feeney, A. (2016). The time course of conflict on the Cognitive Reflection Test. Cognition,150, 109–118. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.015
    Trémolière, B., De Neys, W., & Bonnefon, J. F. (2014). The Grim Reasoner: Analytical Reasoning under Mortality Salience. Thinking & Reasoning, 20, 333-351. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2013.823888
    Trippas, D., Handley, S. J., Verde, M. F., & Morsanyi, K. (2016). Logic brightens my day: Evidence for implicit sensitivity to logical validity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(9), 1448. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000248
    Tsujii, T., & Watanabee, S. (2009). Neural correlates of dual-task effect on belief-bias syllogistic reasoning: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Brain Research, 1287, 118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.06.080
    Tubau, E. (2008). Enhancing probabilistic reasoning: The role of causal graphs, statistical format and numerical skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 187–196. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.08.006
    Tubau, E., Aguilar-Lleyda, D., & Johnson, E. D. (2015). Reasoning and choice in the Monty Hall Dilemma (MHD): Implications for improving Bayesian reasoning. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00353
    Tubau, E. & Alonso, D. (2003). Overcoming illusory inferences in a probabilistic counterintuitive problem: The role of explicit representations. Memory and Cognition, 31, 596–607. doi: 10.3758/BF03196100
    Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(73)90033-9
    Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131. doi: 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124
    Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional vs. intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90, 293–3l5. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.90.4.293
    Villejoubert, G. (2009). Are representativeness judgments automatic and rapid? The effect of time pressure on the conjunction fallacy. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 30, 2980–2985.

    下載圖示
    QR CODE