簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 洪靖淵
Hong, Jing-Yuan
論文名稱: 不同晨間進食狀態下運動對體重過重/肥胖中年人之認知功能影響
Impact of Feeding Status and Exercise Intensity on Cognitive Performance in Adults with Overweight/Obesity
指導教授: 陳勇志
Chen, Yung-Chih
口試委員: 廖翊宏
Liao, Yi-Hung
洪聰敏
Hung, Tsung-Min
陳勇志
Chen, Yung-Chih
口試日期: 2022/01/24
學位類別: 碩士
Master
系所名稱: 體育與運動科學系
Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences
論文出版年: 2022
畢業學年度: 110
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 55
中文關鍵詞: 認知功能執行功能隔夜禁食中年人
英文關鍵詞: Cognition, Executive function, Fasting, Middle-aged adults
研究方法: 實驗設計法
DOI URL: http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202200455
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:214下載:14
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 問題背景:運動強度與早餐進食與否皆能夠影響認知功能,但目前尚不清楚兩者交互作用下的影響。目的:探討有無早餐狀態下進行高 (70% 最大攝氧量) 和中低強度(50% 最大攝氧量)的連續 4 公里有氧運動對體重過重及肥胖健康中年人認知執行功能的影響。方法: 12 名(年齡:40 ± 7 歲、BMI:30 ± 3 kg/m2、腰圍:107 ± 7 cm) 的實驗參與者,經12–14 小時隔夜禁食後,隨機分派至四次實驗;晨間進食下高強度運動(Meal-High)、晨間進食下中低強度運動(Meal-Low)、無晨間進食下高強度運動(Fasted-High)、無晨間進食下中低強度運動(Fasted-Low),電腦化認知功能採用旁側干擾測驗(Flanker task)以及任務轉換測驗(Task switching),於基礎值、運動前和運動後兩小時測驗。結果:研究發現,旁側干擾測驗及任務轉換測驗不論在回應正確率和反應時間,四組間皆無顯著差異 (all, p > 0.05)。以進食狀態 (Fasted and Meal) 及運動強度 (Low and High intensity) 分類資料進行比較,有早餐組一般型任務轉換測驗反應時間快於無早餐組 (p = 0.009);有早餐組混合型任務轉換測驗 ( p = 0.038) 和旁側干擾測驗一致情境 (p = 0.037) 回應準確率優於無早餐組;低強度組混合型任務轉換測驗回應準確率優於高強度組 (p = 0.028)。結論:肥胖、過重中年人進食早餐後進行運動相較於不吃早餐有更佳的執行功能表現;不論進食狀態,中低強度運動相較於高強度運動對認知表現助益較高。

    Background: Breakfast consumption and exercise both have been shown to acutely influence cognitive performances but it is still unclear whether cognition could be affected by the interactive effects of different exercise intensity with and without breakfast intake. Purposes: To investigate the acute effects of high- and low-to-moderate intensity (70% versus 50% of maximum oxygen uptake) exercise with(out) breakfast consumption on cognition in healthy middle-aged adults with central overweight/obesity. Methods: Participants with overweight/obesity completed four trials in a randomized, cross-over fashion; Fasted Low-to-moderate intensity (FL), Meal Low-to-moderate intensity (ML), Fasted High-intensity (FH) and Meal High-intensity (MH). Computerized cognitive tasks included general and mixed task-switching tasks and flanker task were tested at baseline, before exercise and two hours after exercise. Results: There was no difference in any of cognitive performance (i.e., accuracy and reaction time in general and mixed task-switching tasks and flanker task) between 4 trials (all, p > 0.05). Whilst comparing feeding status and exercise intensity, the results showed that the reaction time of the general task-switching test in the group with breakfast was faster than that in the group without breakfast (p = 0.009); the response accuracy of the mixed task-switching (p = 0.038) and flank task (p = 0.037) in the group with breakfast was faster than that in the group without breakfast; The low-intensity group had better response accuracy on the mixed task-switching test than the high-intensity group (p = 0.028). Conclusion: Obese and overweight middle-aged people obtain better executive function performance while consuming breakfast prior to exercise than without breakfast. In the exercise intensity option, low-to-moderate intensity generates better than high intensity.

    謝辭 i 中文摘要 ii 英文摘要 iii 目次 v 圖次 viii 表次 ix 第壹章 緒論 1 第一節 問題背景 1 第二節 研究目的 4 第三節 研究假設 4 第四節 名詞操作型定義 4 第五節 研究限制 5 第貳章 文獻回顧 6 第一節 認知功能與大腦 6 第二節 早餐省略與認知功能 8 第三節 不同進食狀態運動與認知功能 9 第四節 不同強度運動與認知功能 12 第五節 總結 13 第參章 方法與步驟 14 第一節 實驗參與者 14 第二節 實驗時間與地點 15 第三節 實驗流程 15 第四節 研究工具 17 第五節 資料處理與分析 21 第肆章 結果 22 第一節 實驗參與者基本資料 22 第二節 實驗過程生理反應 23 第三節 血糖 24 第四節 認知測驗 26 第五節 視覺量表 32 第六節 相關分析 34 第伍章 討論 35 第一節 任務轉換測驗表現的變化 35 第二節 旁側干擾測驗表現的變化 37 第陸章 研究限制 39 第柒章 結論 40 參考文獻 41 附錄1 研究參與者知情同意書 50 附錄2 健康狀況評估表 51 附錄 3 PAR-Q(身體活動準備問卷) 54 附錄 4 視覺測驗量表(Visual Analogue Scale, VAS) 55

    Ackerman, D. J., &Friedman-Krauss, A. H. (2017). Preschoolers’ Executive Function: Importance, Contributors, Research Needs and Assessment Options. ETS Research Report Series, 2017(1), 1–24. doi.org/10.1002/ets2.12148
    Ammar, A., Trabelsi, K., Boukhris, O., Bouaziz, B., Müller, P., M Glenn, J., …Hökelmann, A. (2020). Effects of Polyphenol-Rich Interventions on Cognition and Brain Health in Healthy Young and Middle-Aged Adults: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 9(5), 1598. doi.org/10.3390/jcm9051598
    Arnsten, A. F. T. (1997). Catecholamine regulation of the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 11(2), 151–162.
    Badhwar, R., Kaur, G., Popli, H., Yadav, D., &Buttar, H. S. (2020). Pathophysiology of Obesity-Related Non-communicable Chronic Diseases and Advancements in Preventive Strategies. In Pathophysiology of Obesity-Induced Health Complications (pp. 317–340). Springer. doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-35358-2_19
    Benau, E. M., Orloff, N. C., Janke, E. A., Serpell, L., &Timko, C. A. (2014). A systematic review of the effects of experimental fasting on cognition. Appetite, 77, 52–61.
    Beydoun, M. A., Beydoun, H. A., Gamaldo, A. A., Teel, A., Zonderman, A. B., &Wang, Y. (2014). Epidemiologic studies of modifiable factors associated with cognition and dementia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health, 14(1), 643. doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-643
    Blair, Clancy; Ursache, A., Blair, C., &Ursache, A. (2011). A bidirectional model of executive functions and self-regulation. Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, 2, 300–320.
    Blair, C., Zelazo, P. D., &Greenberg, M. T. (2005). The measurement of executive function in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 561–571. doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_1
    Boat, R., &Cooper, S. B. (2019). Self-Control and Exercise: A Review of the Bi-Directional Relationship. Brain Plasticity, 5, 97–104. doi.org/10.3233/BPL-190082
    Boksem, M. A. S., Meijman, T. F., &Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: An ERP study. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 107–116. doi.org/doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011
    Borg, G. (1970). Perceived exertion as an indicator of somatic stress. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 2(2), 92–98.
    Borror, A. (2017). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor mediates cognitive improvements following acute exercise. Medical Hypotheses, 106, 1–5. doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2017.06.024
    Bray, G. A., Kim, K. K., &Wilding, J. P. H. (2017). Obesity: a chronic relapsing progressive disease process. A position statement of the World Obesity Federation. Obesity Reviews, 18(7), 715–723. doi.org/10.1111/obr.12551
    Bush, G., Luu, P., &Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215–222. doi.org/doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2
    Catoira, N. P., Tapajóz, F., Allegri, R. F., Lajfer, J., Rodríguez Cámara, M. J., Iturry, M. L., &Castaño, G. O. (2016). Obesity, metabolic profile, and inhibition failure: Young women under scrutiny. Physiology & Behavior, 157, 87–93. doi.org/doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.01.040
    Chan, J. S. Y., Liu, G., Liang, D., Deng, K., Wu, J., &Yan, J. H. (2019). Special Issue – Therapeutic Benefits of Physical Activity for Mood: A Systematic Review on the Effects of Exercise Intensity, Duration, and Modality. The Journal of Psychology, 153(1), 102–125. doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2018.1470487
    Chandrasekaran, B., Pesola, A. J., Rao, C. R., &Arumugam, A. (2021). Does breaking up prolonged sitting improve cognitive functions in sedentary adults? A mapping review and hypothesis formulation on the potential physiological mechanisms. BMC Musculoskeletal Disorders, 22(1), 274. doi.org/10.1186/s12891-021-04136-5
    Chang, Y. K., Labban, J. D., Gapin, J. I., &Etnier, J. L. (2012). The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Research, 1453, 87–101. doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068
    Chang, Yu Kai, Chu, C. H., Wang, C. C., Wang, Y. C., Song, T. F., Tsai, C. L., &Etnier, J. L. (2015). Dose-response relation between exercise duration and cognition. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 47(1), 159–165. doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0000000000000383
    Chung, S. T., Ha, J., Onuzuruike, A. U., Kasturi, K., Galvan-De La Cruz, M., Bingham, B. A., …Sumner, A. E. (2017). Time to glucose peak during an oral glucose tolerance test identifies prediabetes risk. Clinical Endocrinology, 87(5), 484–491. doi.org/doi.org/10.1111/cen.13416
    Coelho, F. G. de M., Gobbi, S., Andreatto, C. A. A., Corazza, D. I., Pedroso, R. V., &Santos-Galduróz, R. F. (2013). Physical exercise modulates peripheral levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF): A systematic review of experimental studies in the elderly. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 56(1), 10–15. doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2012.06.003
    Cohen-Manheim, I., Sinnreich, R., Doniger, G. M., Simon, E. S., Pinchas-Mizrachi, R., &Kark, J. D. (2018). Fasting plasma glucose in young adults free of diabetes is associated with cognitive function in midlife. European Journal of Public Health, 28(3), 496–503. doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckx194
    Cournot, M., Marquié, J. C., Ansiau, D., Martinaud, C., Fonds, H., Ferrières, J., &Ruidavets, J. B. (2006). Relation between body mass index and cognitive function in healthy middle-aged men and women. Neurology, 67(7), 1208–1214. doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000238082.13860.50
    Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
    Dreisbach, G. (2006). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: The costs and benefits of reduced maintenance capability. Brain and Cognition, 60(1), 11–19.
    Edinburgh, R. M., Hengist, A., Smith, H. A., Travers, R. L., Koumanov, F., Betts, J. A., …Gonzalez, J. T. (2018). Preexercise breakfast ingestion versus extended overnight fasting increases postprandial glucose flux after exercise in healthy men. American Journal of Physiology - Endocrinology and Metabolism, 315(5), E1062–E1074. doi.org/10.1152/ajpendo.00163.2018
    Erickson, K. I., Hillman, C., Stillman, C. M., Ballard, R. M., Bloodgood, B., Conroy, D. E., …Powell, K. E. (2019). Physical Activity, Cognition, and Brain Outcomes: A Review of the 2018 Physical Activity Guidelines. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 51(6), 1242–1251. doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0000000000001936
    Eriksen, B. A., &Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149. doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267
    Fiani, B., Zhu, L., Musch, B. L., Briceno, S., Andel, R., Sadeq, N., &Ansari, A. Z. (2021). The Neurophysiology of Caffeine as a Central Nervous System Stimulant and the Resultant Effects on Cognitive Function. Cureus, 13(5), e15032. doi.org/10.7759/cureus.15032
    Galioto, R., &Spitznagel, M. B. (2016). The effects of breakfast and breakfast composition on cognition in adults. Advances in Nutrition, 7(3), 576S-589S. doi.org/10.3945/an.115.010231
    Gibney, M. J., Barr, S. I., Bellisle, F., Drewnowski, A., Fagt, S., Livingstone, B., …Hopkins, S. (2018). Breakfast in human nutrition: The international breakfast research initiative. Nutrients, 10(5), 559. doi.org/10.3390/nu10050559
    Heni, M., Kullmann, S., Ketterer, C., Guthoff, M., Bayer, M., Staiger, H., …Fritsche, A. (2014). Differential effect of glucose ingestion on the neural processing of food stimuli in lean and overweight adults. Human Brain Mapping, 35(3), 918–928. doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22223
    Hüttermann, S., &Memmert, D. (2014). Does the inverted-U function disappear in expert athletes? An analysis of the attentional behavior under physical exercise of athletes and non-athletes. Physiology & Behavior, 131, 87–92. doi.org/doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.04.020
    Hyafil, A., Summerfield, C., &Koechlin, E. (2009). Two mechanisms for task switching in the prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(16), 5135–5142. doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2828-08.2009
    Jakubowicz, D., Wainstein, J., Landau, Z., Raz, I., Ahren, B., Chapnik, N., …Froy, O. (2017). Influences of Breakfast on Clock Gene Expression and Postprandial Glycemia in Healthy Individuals and Individuals With Diabetes: A Randomized Clinical Trial. Diabetes Care, 40(11), 1573 LP – 1579. doi.org/10.2337/dc16-2753
    Kaur, S., Gonzales, M. M., Tarumi, T., Villalpando, A., Alkatan, M., Pyron, M., …Haley, A. P. (2016). Serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor mediates the relationship between abdominal adiposity and executive function in middle age. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22(5), 493–500. doi.org/10.1017/S1355617716000230
    Komiyama, T., Sudo, M., Okuda, N., Yasuno, T., Kiyonaga, A., Tanaka, H., …Ando, S. (2016). Cognitive function at rest and during exercise following breakfast omission. Physiology and Behavior, 157, 178–184. doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.02.013
    Kotopoulea-Nikolaidi, M., Watkins, E., &Giannopoulou, I. (2019). Effects of high carbohydrate vs. High protein pre-exercise feedings on psychophysiological responses to high intensity interval exercise in overweight perimenopausal women. Frontiers in Nutrition, 5, 141. doi.org/10.3389/fnut.2018.00141
    Kullmann, S., Heni, M., Hallschmid, M., Fritsche, A., Preissl, H., &Häring, H.-U. (2016). Brain Insulin Resistance at the Crossroads of Metabolic and Cognitive Disorders in Humans. Physiological Reviews, 96(4), 1169–1209. doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00032.2015
    Lamport, Daniel J., Lawton, C. L., Mansfield, M. W., Moulin, C. A. J., &Dye, L. (2014). Type 2 diabetes and impaired glucose tolerance are associated with word memory source monitoring recollection deficits but not simple recognition familiarity deficits following water, low glycaemic load, and high glycaemic load breakfasts. Physiology and Behavior, 124, 54–60. doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.10.033
    Lamport, Daniel Joseph, Hoyle, E., Lawton, C. L., Mansfield, M. W., &Dye, L. (2011). Evidence for a second meal cognitive effect: Glycaemic responses to high and low glycaemic index evening meals are associated with cognition the following morning. Nutritional Neuroscience, 14(2), 66–71. doi.org/10.1179/1476830511Y.0000000002
    Leckie, R. L., Oberlin, L. E., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Szabo-Reed, A., Chaddock-Heyman, L., …Erickson, K. I. (2014). BDNF mediates improvements in executive function following a 1-year exercise intervention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(DEC). doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00985
    Liu, Z., Zaid, M., Hisamatsu, T., Tanaka, S., Fujiyoshi, A., Miyagawa, N., …Ueshima, H. (2020). Elevated fasting blood glucose levels are associated with lower cognitive function, with a threshold in non-diabetic individuals: A population-based study. Journal of Epidemiology, 30(3), 121–127. doi.org/10.2188/jea.JE20180193
    Livingston, G., Sommerlad, A., Orgeta, V., Costafreda, S. G., Huntley, J., Ames, D., …Mukadam, N. (2017). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. The Lancet, 390(10113), 2673–2734. doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6
    Lommatzsch, M., Zingler, D., Schuhbaeck, K., Schloetcke, K., Zingler, C., Schuff-Werner, P., &Virchow, J. C. (2005). The impact of age, weight and gender on BDNF levels in human platelets and plasma. Neurobiology of Aging, 26(1), 115–123. doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.03.002
    Ma, X., Chen, Q., Pu, Y., Guo, M., Jiang, Z., Huang, W., …Xu, Y. (2020). Skipping breakfast is associated with overweight and obesity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity Research and Clinical Practice, 14(1), 1–8. doi.org/10.1016/j.orcp.2019.12.002
    Marosi, K., &Mattson, M. P. (2014). BDNF mediates adaptive brain and body responses to energetic challenges. Trends in Endocrinology and Metabolism, 25(2), 89–98. doi.org/10.1016/j.tem.2013.10.006
    Mattson, M. P., Moehl, K., Ghena, N., Schmaedick, M., &Cheng, A. (2018). Intermittent metabolic switching, neuroplasticity and brain health. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 19(2), 63–80. doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.156
    McMorris, T., &Hale, B. J. (2012). Differential effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy of cognition: A meta-analytical investigation. Brain and Cognition, 80(3), 338–351. doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2012.09.001
    Meeusen, R., VanCutsem, J., &Roelands, B. (2020). Endurance exercise-induced and mental fatigue and the brain. Experimental Physiology. doi.org/10.1113/EP088186
    Mehren, A., Diaz Luque, C., Brandes, M., Lam, A. P., Thiel, C. M., Philipsen, A., &Özyurt, J. (2019). Intensity-Dependent Effects of Acute Exercise on Executive Function. Neural Plasticity, 2019, 8608317. doi.org/10.1155/2019/8608317
    Mitchell, R. L. C., &Phillips, L. H. (2007). The psychological, neurochemical and functional neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of positive and negative mood on executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 617–629. doi.org/doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.030
    Mohammed, A.-R., &Lyusin, D. (2021). The effect of an induced negative mood on the updating of affective information. Cognitive Processing. doi.org/10.1007/s10339-021-01060-3
    Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140. doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7
    Moore, R. D., Romine, M. W., O’Connor, P. J., &Tomporowski, P. D. (2012). The influence of exercise-induced fatigue on cognitive function. Journal of Sports Sciences, 30(9), 841–850. doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2012.675083
    Newman, J. C., &Verdin, E. (2014). Ketone bodies as signaling metabolites. Trends in Endocrinology and Metabolism, 25(1), 42–52. doi.org/10.1016/j.tem.2013.09.002
    Nilsson, A., Radeborg, K., &Björck, I. (2009). Effects of differences in postprandial glycaemia on cognitive functions in healthy middle-aged subjects. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 63(1), 113–120. doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602900
    Nilsson, A., Radeborg, K., &Björck, I. (2012). Effects on cognitive performance of modulating the postprandial blood glucose profile at breakfast. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 66(9), 1039–1043. doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2012.80
    O’Donovan, G., Stamatakis, E., Stensel, D. J., &Hamer, M. (2018). The Importance of Vigorous-Intensity Leisure-Time Physical Activity in Reducing Cardiovascular Disease Mortality Risk in the Obese. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 93(8), 1096–1103. Elsevier. doi.org/10.1016/j.mayocp.2018.01.016
    Page, K. A., Chan, O., Arora, J., Belfort-Deaguiar, R., Dzuira, J., Roehmholdt, B., …Sherwin, R. S. (2013). Effects of fructose vs glucose on regional cerebral blood flow in brain regions involved with appetite and reward pathways. JAMA, 309(1), 63–70. doi.org/10.1001/jama.2012.116975
    Paoli, A., Tinsley, G., Bianco, A., &Moro, T. (2019). The influence of meal frequency and timing on health in humans: The role of fasting. Nutrients, 11(4), 719. doi.org/10.3390/nu11040719
    Paul, G. L., Rokusek, J. T., Dykstra, G. L., Boileau, R. A., &Layman, D. K. (1996). Preexercise meal composition alters plasma large neutral amino acid responses during exercise and recovery. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 64(5), 778–786. doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/64.5.778
    Pedersen, M., Pedersen, K. K., Bruunsgaard, H., Krabbe, K. S., Thomsen, C., Færch, K., …Mortensen, E. L. (2012). Cognitive Functions in Middle Aged Individuals Are Related to Metabolic Disturbances and Aerobic Capacity: A Cross-Sectional Study. PLoS ONE, 7(12), e51132. doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051132
    Phillips, L. H., Bull, R., Adams, E., &Fraser, L. (2002). Positive mood and executive function: Evidence from Stroop and fluency tasks. Emotion, Vol. 2, pp. 12–22. Phillips, Louise H.: U Aberdeen, Dept of Psychology, Aberdeen, Scotland, AB24 2UB, louise.phillips@abdn.ac.uk: American Psychological Association. doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.2.1.12
    Pillai, A., Bruno, D., Sarreal, A. S., Hernando, R. T., Saint-Louis, L. A., Nierenberg, J., …Buckley, P. F. (2012). Plasma BDNF levels vary in relation to body weight in females. PLoS ONE, 7(7), e39358. doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0039358
    Ralston, J., Brinsden, H., Buse, K., Candeias, V., Caterson, I., Hassell, T., …Woodward, E. (2018). Time for a new obesity narrative. The Lancet, 392(10156), 1384–1386. doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32537-6
    Sanchez-Aguadero, N., Recio-Rodriguez, J. I., Patino-Alonso, M. C., Mora-Simon, S., Alonso-Dominguez, R., Sanchez-Salgado, B., …Garcia-Ortiz, L. (2020). Postprandial effects of breakfast glycaemic index on cognitive performance among young, healthy adults: A crossover clinical trial. Nutritional Neuroscience, 23(1), 1–7. doi.org/10.1080/1028415X.2018.1461459
    Santos, H. O., Genario, R., Macedo, R. C. O., Pareek, M., &Tinsley, G. M. (2020). Association of breakfast skipping with cardiovascular outcomes and cardiometabolic risk factors: an updated review of clinical evidence. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition, 1–9. doi.org/10.1080/10408398.2020.1819768
    Seamans, J. K., &Yang, C. R. (2004). The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortex. Progress in Neurobiology, 74(1), 1–58.
    Si, J., Zhang, H., Zhu, L., &Chen, A. (2021). The relationship between overweight/obesity and executive control in college students: The mediating effect of BDNF and 5-HT. Life, 11(4), 313. doi.org/10.3390/life11040313
    Smith, E., Hay, P., Campbell, L., &Trollor, J. N. (2011). A review of the association between obesity and cognitive function across the lifespan: Implications for novel approaches to prevention and treatment. Obesity Reviews, 12(9), 740–755. doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00920.x
    Solianik, R., &Sujeta, A. (2018). Two-day fasting evokes stress, but does not affect mood, brain activity, cognitive, psychomotor, and motor performance in overweight women. Behavioural Brain Research, 338, 166–172. doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2017.10.028
    Solianik, R., Sujeta, A., Terentjeviene, A., &Skurvydas, A. (2016). Effect of 48h fasting on autonomic function, brain activity, cognition, and mood in amateur weight lifters. BioMed Research International, 2016, 1503956. doi.org/10.1155/2016/1503956
    Sun, M. K. (2018). Executive functioning: Perspectives on neurotrophic activity and pharmacology. Behavioural Pharmacology, 29(7), 592–604. doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0000000000000427
    Sünram-Lea, S. I., &Owen, L. (2017). The impact of diet-based glycaemic response and glucose regulation on cognition: Evidence across the lifespan. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 76(4), 466–477. doi.org/10.1017/S0029665117000829
    Thomas, S. N., Schroeder, T., Secher, N. H., &Mitchell, J. H. (1989). Cerebral blood flow during submaximal and maximal dynamic exercise in humans. Journal of Applied Physiology, 67(2), 744–748. doi.org/10.1152/jappl.1989.67.2.744
    Tsukamoto, H., Ishibashi, A., Marley, C. J., Shinohara, Y., Ando, S., Bailey, D. M., …Ogoh, S. (2021). Plasma brain-derived neurotrophic factor and dynamic cerebral autoregulation in acute response to glycemic control following breakfast in young men. American Journal of Physiology - Regulatory Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 320(1), R69–R79. doi.org/10.1152/AJPREGU.00059.2020
    Vaynman, S., Ying, Z., &Gomez-Pinilla, F. (2007). The select action of hippocampal calcium calmodulin protein kinase II in mediating exercise-enhanced cognitive function. Neuroscience, 144(3), 825–833. doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.10.005
    Veasey, R. C., Gonzalez, J. T., Kennedy, D. O., Haskell, C. F., &Stevenson, E. J. (2013). Breakfast consumption and exercise interact to affect cognitive performance and mood later in the day. a randomized controlled trial. Appetite, 68, 38–44. doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.04.011
    Veasey, Rachel C., Haskell-Ramsay, C. F., Kennedy, D. O., Tiplady, B., &Stevenson, E. J. (2015). The effect of breakfast prior to morning exercise on cognitive performance, mood and appetite later in the day in habitually active women. Nutrients, 7(7), 5712–5732. doi.org/10.3390/nu7075250
    Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Telang, F., Fowler, J. S., Goldstein, R. Z., Alia-Klein, N., …Pradhan, K. (2009). Inverse association between BMI and prefrontal metabolic activity in healthy adults. Obesity (Silver Spring, Md.), 17(1), 60–65. doi.org/10.1038/oby.2008.469
    WHO Expert Consultation. (2011). Waist Circumference and Waist-Hip Ratio Report of a WHO Expert Consultation, Geneva, 8-11 December 2008 (pp. 8–11). pp. 8–11. Geneva: World Health Organization. Retrieved from apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/44583
    Wolff, W., Sieber, V., Bieleke, M., &Englert, C. (2021). Task duration and task order do not matter: no effect on self-control performance. Psychological Research, 85(1), 397–407. doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01230-1
    Wrann, C. D., White, J. P., Salogiannnis, J., Laznik-Bogoslavski, D., Wu, J., Ma, D., …Spiegelman, B. M. (2013). Exercise Induces Hippocampal BDNF through a PGC-1α/FNDC5 Pathway. Cell Metabolism, 18(5), 649–659. doi.org/doi.org/10.1016/j.cmet.2013.09.008
    Zigmond, A. S., &Snaith, R. P. (1983). The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67(6), 361–370. doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

    下載圖示
    QR CODE