研究生: |
曾楷翔 Tseng, Kai-Hsiang |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
不同科學能力的四年級學生閱讀科學漫畫時的理解歷程與態度之眼動研究 Understanding and Attitudes of Fourth-grade Students with Different Scientific Abilities When Reading Science Comics: An Eye Movement Study |
指導教授: |
簡郁芩
Jian, Yu-Cin |
口試委員: |
曾世杰
Tzeng, Shih-Jay 吳昭容 Wu, Chao-Jung 簡郁芩 Jian, Yu-Cin |
口試日期: | 2024/01/04 |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
教育心理與輔導學系 Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling |
論文出版年: | 2024 |
畢業學年度: | 112 |
語文別: | 中文 |
論文頁數: | 91 |
中文關鍵詞: | 科學態度 、科學漫畫 、眼動追蹤 、閱讀理解 |
英文關鍵詞: | scientific attitude, science comics, eye-tracking, reading comprehension |
研究方法: | 實驗設計法 |
DOI URL: | http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202400146 |
論文種類: | 學術論文 |
相關次數: | 點閱:118 下載:9 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
科學漫畫除了娛樂效果外,也是學習自然科學的一種方式。綜觀過去對於科學漫畫的研究,多數注重閱讀漫畫後的理解效果,較少關注讀者透過閱讀漫畫來學習科學知識的歷程與情意態度,且四年級學生正面臨閱讀發展階段的轉換,閱讀歷程會與以往不同。因此,本研究冀由眼動追蹤的方式探討不同科學能力的四年級學生在閱讀科學漫畫的認知歷程與處理策略,並以問卷了解學生對於閱讀科學漫畫後的態度。本研究的64位效樣本為國小四年級生,藉由自然科學三個學期平均成績作為科學能力分組,均閱讀一篇與科學知識相關的純文字文本、一篇漫畫文本。主要結果有三點:第一,高科學能力的讀者在閱讀理解後測表現優於低科學能力讀者;兩組在閱讀科學漫畫與科學純文本的學習材料閱讀理解並無差異,顯示科學漫畫與純文本材料在知識傳遞程度上相當。第二,閱讀完科學漫畫文本後,高、低科學能力的學生對自然科學態度均有明顯提升。第三,比較閱讀漫畫材料與閱讀文字材料的眼動數據,高科學能力學生讀文字材料與漫畫材料皆比低科學能力學生花較少的認知資源。比較兩組閱讀漫畫的細部指標,本研究發現低科學能力學生閱讀漫畫時會花較多時間閱讀與科學知識較不相關的訊息、高科學能力學生在閱讀漫畫時文字解碼較為流暢且在第一次閱讀時就可以理解文章所傳達之知識內容,所以重讀行為較少,且高科學能力者較低科學能力者較能從漫畫中找到與科學相關的重點,並進而分配注意力與認知資源在其中。綜合上述,本研究深入剖析小學四年級學生閱讀科學漫畫的歷程,有助於我們更全面地理解科學漫畫對小學四年級學生學習科學的影響。
Science comics, apart from providing entertainment, serve as a unique means of learning natural sciences. Previous research on science comics has primarily focused on post-reading comprehension effects, with less attention given to understanding readers' cognitive processes and emotional attitudes as they learn scientific knowledge through comics. Additionally, fourth-grade students undergo a critical stage in their reading development, where their reading processes differ from earlier stages. This study aims to investigate the cognitive processes and processing strategies of fourth-grade students with varying scientific abilities in reading science comics using eye-tracking technology. A questionnaire was also employed to assess students' attitudes toward reading science comics. The study, comprising 64 participants from fourth-grade elementary school, utilized average scores from three semesters of natural science to group students based on scientific ability. Participants read both a plain text document related to scientific knowledge and a comic. The main findings include: first, readers with higher scientific abilities performed better in the post-reading comprehension test compared to those with lower scientific abilities. Both groups showed no significant difference in reading comprehension between science comics and plain text materials, indicating comparable knowledge transmission levels. Second, after reading science comic, students with both high and low scientific abilities exhibited a significant improvement in their attitudes toward natural science. Third, when comparing eye-tracking data between reading comic and plain text materials, students with higher scientific abilities allocated fewer cognitive resources to both text and comic materials compared to those with lower scientific abilities. Analyzing detailed indicators of comic reading, the study found that lower scientific ability students spent more time reading information unrelated to scientific knowledge, while higher scientific ability students exhibited smoother text decoding during comic reading and comprehended the conveyed knowledge content on their first read, resulting in fewer instances of re-reading. Furthermore, students with higher scientific abilities were more adept at identifying science-related key points within the comics, allocating attention and cognitive resources accordingly. In summary, this study provides a thorough analysis of the reading process of science comics for fourth-grade students, contributing to a comprehensive understanding of the impact of science comics on the learning of science by this age group.
王孜甯、簡郁芩(2022):〈科學圖文閱讀眼動研究之系統回顧〉。《教育心理學報》,53,773–799。https://doi.org/10.6251/BEP.202206_53(4).0001
行政院文化部(2015):《102年暨103年臺灣出版產業調查報告》。
https://teric.naer.edu.tw/wSite/ct?ctNode=648&mp=teric_b&xItem=1900395&resCtNode=648&OWASP_CSRFTOKEN=WAB3-UZS8-7V5V-8U5M-EBHT-ATTQ-E7AD-UA7
行政院文化部(2019):《107年臺灣民眾閱讀及消費行為調查報告》。
https://stat.moc.gov.tw/research.aspx?type=4
行政院教育部(2018):《十二年國民基本教育自然科學領域課程綱要》。
https://ghresource.k12ea.gov.tw/uploads/1613715832381Ld8uk4KU.pdf
李新鄉、盧姿里、黃光明(2014):〈國小自然與生活科技領域學習低成就狀況及採STS教學策略進行補救教學成效研究〉。《南台人文社會學報》,11,33–68。
柯華葳、詹益綾(2006):《國民小學(二至六年級)閱讀理解篩選測驗》。教育部特殊教育小組。
國立臺灣師範大學科學教育中心(2018):《國際數學與科學教育成就趨勢調查2015國家報告》。
張翔皓(2010):《發展科學漫畫教材對國小四年級學童科學學習成效之研究》(未出版碩士論文),國立臺北教育大學。
曾世杰(計畫主持人)(2018):《漫畫式國語文補救教材之可行性與成效評估:對國小中年級弱勢低成就學生的真實驗研究》(計畫編號:MOST106-2410-H143-006)。國科會補助專題研究計畫成果報告,國科會。
https://www.grb.gov.tw/search/planDetail?id=12237555
曾世杰、陳淑麗(2020):〈以漫畫提升二年級語文低成就兒童的中文閱讀理解〉。《課程與教學季刊》,23,129–152。https://doi.org/10.6384/CIQ.202004_23(2).0006
簡郁芩(2006):《以眼動型態探討背景知識對詞彙辨識的影響》(未出版碩士論文),國立中央大學。
簡郁芩(2013):《從圖文閱讀的眼動型態建構與驗證機械動態表徵的認知模式》(未出版博士論文),國立臺灣師範大學。
Baddeley, A., Logie, R., Bressi, S., Sala, S. D., & Spinnler, H. (1986). Dementia and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 38(4), 603–618. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748608401616
Bennett, J., Lubben, F., & Hogarth, S. (2007). Bringing science to life: A synthesis of the research evidence on the effects of context‐based and STS approaches to science teaching. Science Education, 91(3), 347–370.
https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20186
Brehm-Heeger, P., Conway, A., & Vale, C. (2007). Cosplay, gaming, and conventions: The amazing and unexpected places an anime club can lead unsuspecting librarians. Young Adult Library Services, 5(2), 14–16.
Brookshire, J., Scharff, L. F., & Moses, L. E. (2002). The influence of illustrations on children's book preferences and comprehension. Reading Psychology, 23(4), 323–339. https://doi.org/10.1080/713775287
Butcher, K. R. (2014). The multimedia principle. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 174–205). Cambridge University Press.
Caillies, S., Denhière, G., & Kintsch, W. (2002). The effect of prior knowledge on understanding from text: Evidence from primed recognition. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14(2), 267–286.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440143000069
Chall, J. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. McGraw Hill.
Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 8(4), 293–332. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0804_2
Chiaverina, C., Scott, C., & Steele, P. (1997). The connections project: Art, physics, and mathematics. The Physics Teacher, 35(5), 292–294.
Chudapongse, P., & Haugaard, N. (1973). The effect of phosphoenolypyruvate on calcium transport by mitochondria. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta (BBA)-Biomembranes, 307(3), 599–606. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-2736(73)90304-0
Cook, M., Wiebe, E. N., & Carter, G. (2008). The influence of prior knowledge on viewing and interpreting graphics with macroscopic and molecular representations. Science Education, 92(5), 848–867.
https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20262
Duchastel, P. C. (1981). Retention of prose following testing with different types of tests. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 6(3), 217–226.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0361-476X(81)90002-3
Eitel, A. (2016). How repeated studying and testing affects multimedia learning: Evidence for adaptation to task demands. Learning and Instruction, 41, 70–84.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.10.003
Fletcher, J. D., & Tobias, S. (2005). The multimedia principle. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 117–134). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816819.008
Gerde, V. W., & Foster, R. S. (2008). X-Men ethics: Using comic books to teach business ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 77, 245–258.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-006-9347-3
Green, M. J., & Myers, K. R. (2010). Graphic medicine: Use of comics in medical education and patient care. Bmj, 340, 574–577. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.c863
Hannus, M., & Hyönä, J. (1999). Utilization of illustrations during learning of science textbook passages among low-and high-ability children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24(2), 95–123.
Hegarty, M. (1992). Mental animation: Inferring motion from static displays of mechanical systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(5), 1084–1102.
Hegarty, M., & Just, M. A. (1993). Constructing mental models of machines from text and diagrams. Journal of Memory and Language, 32(6), 717–742.
Hughes‐Hassell, S., & Rodge, P. (2007). The leisure reading habits of urban adolescents. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 51(1), 22–33.
https://doi.org/10.1598/JAAL.51.1.3
Hung, Y. N. (2014). “What are you looking at?” An eye movement exploration in science text reading. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 12, 241–260. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-013-9406-z
Hosler, J., & Boomer, K. B. (2011). Are comic books an effective way to engage nonmajors in learning and appreciating science? CBE—Life Sciences Education, 10(3), 309–317. https://doi.org/10.1187/cbe.10-07-0090
Jian, Y. C. (2017). Eye-movement patterns and reader characteristics of students with good and poor performance when reading scientific text with diagrams. Reading and Writing, 30(7), 1447–1472. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9732-6
Jian, Y. C. (2019). Reading instructions facilitate signaling effect on science text for young readers: An eye-movement study. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 17, 503–522.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-018-9878-y
Jian, Y. C. (2020). Teaching fourth-grade students of different reading abilities to read biological illustrations and integrate in-text information: An empirical experiment. Research in Science Education, 50(6), 2269–2282.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-018-9778-8
Jian, Y. C. (2023). Reading Behavior in Science Comics and Its Relations with Comprehension Performance and Reading Attitudes: An Eye-tracker Study. Research in Science Education, 53(4), 689–706.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-022-10093-3
Jian, Y. C., Chen, M. L., & Ko, H. W. (2013). Context Effects in Processing of Chinese Academic Words: An Eye‐Tracking Investigation. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(4), 403–413. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.56
Jian, Y. C., & Ko, H. W. (2014). Investigating the effects of background knowledge on Chinese word processing during text reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Research in Reading, 37(S1), S71–S86.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01534.x
Jian, Y. C., & Ko, H. W. (2017). Influences of text difficulty and reading ability on learning illustrated science texts for children: An eye movement study. Computers & Education, 113, 263–279.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2017.06.002
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87(4), 329–354.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.87.4.329
Kakalios, J. (2002). Adding Pow! to your physics class with comic-book lessons. Curriculum Review, 42(2), 14–15.
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163–182.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.95.2.163
Lenzner, A., Schnotz, W., & Müller, A. (2013). The role of decorative pictures in learning. Instructional Science, 41, 811–831.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-012-9256-z
Lindner, M. A., Eitel, A., Strobel, B., & Köller, O. (2017). Identifying processes underlying the multimedia effect in testing: An eye-movement analysis. Learning and Instruction, 47, 91–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.10.007
Lin, S. F., Lin, H. S., Lee, L., & Yore, L. D. (2015). Are science comics a good medium for science communication? The case for public learning of nanotechnology. International Journal of Science Education, Part B, 5(3), 276–294. https://doi.org/10.1080/21548455.2014.941040
Makransky, G., Terkildsen, T. S., & Mayer, R. E. (2019). Role of subjective and objective measures of cognitive processing during learning in explaining the spatial contiguity effect. Learning and Instruction, 61, 23–34.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.12.001
Male, A. (2023). Illustration: A theoretical and contextual perspective. Bloomsbury Publishing. https://doi.org/10.5040/9781474263054
Martín Arnal, L. A., León, J. A., Broek, P. V. D., & Olmos, R. (2019). Understanding comics: A comparison between children and adults through a coherence/incoherence paradigm in an eye-tracking study. Psicología Educativa, 25, 127–137.
Mason, L., Pluchino, P., Tornatora, M. C., & Ariasi, N. (2013). An eye-tracking study of learning from science text with concrete and abstract illustrations. The Journal of Experimental Education, 81(3), 356–384.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2012.727885
Mayer, R. E. (1997). Multimedia learning: Are we asking the right questions? Educational Psychologist, 32(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3201_1
Mayer, R. E. (2005). The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning. Cambridge university press.
Means, M. L., & Voss, J. F. (1985). Star Wars: A developmental study of expert and novice knowledge structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 24(6), 746–757.
Merc, A., & Kampusu, Y. (2013). The effect of comic strips on EFL reading comprehension. International Journal on New Trends in Education and Their Implications, 4(1), 54–64.
Morrow, D., D’andrea, L., Stine-Morrow, E. A., Shake, M., Bertel, S., Chin, J., Kopren, K., Gao, X., Conner-Garcia, T., Graumlich, J., & Murray, M. (2012). Comprehension of multimedia health information among older adults with chronic illness. Visual Communication, 11(3), 347–362.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1470357212446413
Nakazawa, J. (2005). Development of manga (comic book) literacy in children. In D. W. Shwalb, J. Nakazawa, & B. J. Shwalb (Eds.), Applied developmental psychology: Theory, practice, and research from Japan (pp. 23–42). Information Age Publishing.
National Research Council. (2014). Literacy for science: Exploring the intersection of the Next Generation Science Standards and Common Core for ELA standards: A workshop summary. National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/18803
Paivio, A. (1990). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford university press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195066661.003.0004
Perie, M., Grigg, W., & Dion, G. (2005). The nation's report card: Mathematics 2005 (NCES 2006–453). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.
Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372
Rayner, K., Chace, K. H., Slattery, T. J., & Ashby, J. (2006). Eye movements as reflections of comprehension processes in reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(3), 241–255. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr1003_3
Readence, J. E., & Moore, D. W. (1981). A meta‐analytic review of the effect of adjunct pictures on reading comprehension. Psychology in the Schools, 18(2), 218–224.
https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(198104)18:2<218::AID-PITS2310180219>3.0.CO;2-1
Rubens, P. (2002). Science and technical writing: A manual of style. Routledge.
Rota, G., & Izquierdo, J. (2003). “Comics” as a tool for teaching biotechnology in primary schools. Electronic Journal of Biotechnology, 6(2), 85–89.
https://doi.org/10.2225/vol6-issue2-fulltext-10
Schnotz, W., & Bannert, M. (2003). Construction and interference in learning from multiple representation. Learning and Instruction, 13(2), 141–156.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00017-8
Singh, K., Granville, M., & Dika, S. (2002). Mathematics and science achievement: Effects of motivation, interest, and academic engagement. The Journal of Educational Research, 95(6), 323–332.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00220670209596607
Sjøberg, S., & Schreiner, C. (2012). A Comparative View on Adolescents’ Attitudes towards Science1. In M. W. Bauer, R. Shukla, & N. Allum (Eds.), The culture of science: How the public relates to science across the globe (pp. 200–216). Routledge.
Sones, W. W. D. (1944). The comics and instructional method. The Journal of Educational Sociology, 18(4), 232–240. https://doi.org/10.2307/2262696
Spiegel, A. N., McQuillan, J., Halpin, P., Matuk, C., & Diamond, J. (2013). Engaging teenagers with science through comics. Research in Science Education, 43, 2309–2326. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-013-9358-x
Szabo, M., Dwyer, F. M., & DeMelo, H. (1981). Visual testing—Visual literacy’s second dimension. ECTJ, 29(3), 177–187. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02766520
Tabassum, M., Alqhatani, A., Aldossari, M., & Richter Lipford, H. (2018, April 21–26). Increasing user attention with a comic-based policy [Paper presentation]. The 2018 Chi Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Montréal, QC.
Takahashi, S. (1995). Aesthetic properties of pictorial perception. Psychological Review, 102(4), 671–683.
Tatalovic, M. (2009). Science comics as tools for science education and communication: A brief, exploratory study. Journal of Science Communication, 8(04), A02. https://doi.org/10.22323/2.08040202
Snyder, T. D., & Dillow, S. A. (2015). Digest of education statistics (NCES 2015–011). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.
https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2015/2015011_1.pdf
Voss, J. F., Vesonder, G. T., & Spilich, G. J. (1980). Text generation and recall by high-knowledge and low-knowledge individuals. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(6), 651–667.
Weitkamp, E., & Burnet, F. (2007). The Chemedian brings laughter to the chemistry classroom. International Journal of Science Education, 29(15), 1911–1929.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09500690701222790
Williams, N. (1995). The comic book as course book: Why and how. TESOL Convention.
Worthy, J., Moorman, M., & Turner, M. (1999). What Johnny likes to read is hard to find in school. Reading Research Quarterly, 34(1), 12–27.
https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.34.1.2
Zhao, F., & Mahrt, N. (2018). Influences of comics expertise and comics types in comics reading. International Journal of Innovation and Research in Educational Sciences, 5(2), 218–224.