簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 李定武
Lee, Ting-Wu
論文名稱: 漢語空語類的相關腦區探究
Neural Correlates of Empty Categories in Mandarin Chinese
指導教授: 詹曉蕙
Chan, Shiao-Hui
口試委員: 詹曉蕙
Chan, Shiao-Hui
何萬順
Her, One-Soon
周泰立
Chou, Tai-Li
徐峻賢
Hsu, Chun-Hsien
吳曉虹
Wu, Hsiao-Hung
口試日期: 2023/06/29
學位類別: 博士
Doctor
系所名稱: 英語學系
Department of English
論文出版年: 2023
畢業學年度: 111
語文別: 英文
論文頁數: 214
中文關鍵詞: 空語類漢語句法移位磁振造影左額下回左後顳中回
英文關鍵詞: empty category, Mandarin Chinese, syntactic movement, fMRI, left inferior frontal gyrus, left posterior middle gyrus
研究方法: 實驗設計法
DOI URL: http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202300712
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:109下載:8
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 傳統句法理論將空語類(empty categories)分為四類:名詞軌跡(NP-trace)、疑問詞軌跡(wh-trace)、小隱性代詞(pro)與大隱性代詞(PRO)。空語類各受制於不同的句法限制,也有不同的分布,但這些類別只是理論需要,抑或是人類在句子處理時在行為上、心理上、腦神經上能有相關之反應,包含辨識其空位(gap)及填充語(filler),並且在空位還原填充語等,都有極大的爭議。雖然行為實驗、事件相關電位實驗,甚至功能性磁振造影實驗對填充語-空位依存關係已有了一定程度的了解,然而包含空語類之間的腦神經機制比較,以及空語類最根本的神經特質為何,卻缺乏全面性的探討。本論文透過三個功能性磁振造影實驗探討形式句法理論中空語類的腦神經基底,了解大腦如何辨識與處理空語類,進而理解大腦處理句法移位的相關腦神經機制。
    實驗一比較漢語主題結構與關係子句,希望能從填充語-空位的相對順序了解大腦在處理移位方向的差異。實驗結果發現「填充語先行」與「空位先行」的兩種順序皆會引起大腦左額葉下迴(left inferior frontal gyrus) 以及左顳葉上/中迴 (left superior/middle temporal gyrus)的反應,表示該兩個腦區對移位處理至關重要,但對於移動方向則不敏感。然而關係子句的空位先行卻額外誘發了左前顳葉的反應(LaTL),我們主張該腦區所扮演之角色為借助窺探動詞語意,以推敲空位之語意與詞彙內容,暫行完成動詞片語結構,才能繼續句子處理,預測填充語之存在。
    第二個實驗比較主題結構與被字句結構,進而了解帶有空位的不同結構是否有類似的處理。過去理論研究認為漢語被字句結構並非如英文一般的派生方式(A-movement),留下名詞軌跡;但漢語的長被字句被認為帶有一個原生(base-generated)的受事者主詞,伴隨結構後方的隱性空算符移動 (null operator movement)。然而本實驗卻發現被字句處理與主題結構處理極為相似,同樣引起左額葉下迴與左顳葉中迴的反應,因此並無法支持「隱性移動」或者「不移動」的理論假設。但也因此,本實驗無法直接證實名詞軌跡與疑問詞軌跡之間有處理歷程上的差異。
    第三個實驗比較漢語當中希望類動詞與嘗試類動詞所引導的空主詞子句,其中前者帶有小隱性代詞,後者帶有大隱性代詞,兩者依空語類理論有不同的分布。而根據實驗結果我們發現,小隱性代詞能激發左額葉下迴前端,大隱性代詞則沒有顯著的腦區反應,也反應了兩者處理的難易與差異。左額葉下迴前端的反應可能牽涉到處理小隱性代詞需汲取先行詞的訊息,然而由於小隱性代詞的處理過程並不透過移位,因此未能誘發出前兩個實驗的左額葉下迴的活動。此外,被字句結構與希望類動詞結構同樣都引起右腦前額葉的反應,此反應或許與結構本身有關,因為兩種句構的空語類位子皆可以有空語類、顯性代名詞、名詞等可能性,但三者的最自然指涉又恰好不同,因此引起受試者額外的判斷與決定,這也是本論文較為意外的發現。
    三個實驗結果似乎部分支持空語類框架,因為隱性代詞彼此之間、隱性代詞與軌跡之間處理同中有異;但也因此無法全面支持空語類框架的四分法,例如本實驗無法找出名詞軌跡與疑問詞軌跡之腦神經處理機制的差異。
    整體來說,我們認為左額葉下迴在句子理解的主要功能為工作記憶,在辨識填充語或空位時,能以額外的工作記憶儲存,保持,並予以釋出;左顳葉上/中迴主要功能為整合:整合填充語與空位,空位與動詞,以利句子建構。但左額葉下迴與左顳葉上/中迴的反覆出現,反映了空語類除了「空」以外最重要的基底:依存性。依存性需要透過兩個腦區的共同處理:左額葉下迴處理儲存與汲取結構中被依賴的成分(無論能否被預先辨識出),而左顳葉上/中迴進一步整合空語類所需之語意、語類、句法訊息對於先行詞/填充詞之依賴。此推測能夠進一步解釋兩個腦區對於其他具有依存性的結構的反應,包含代名詞,反身代名詞,以及主詞-動詞呼應等。

    This dissertation explored the neural correlates of empty categories (ECs) in Generative Grammar. Traditionally, the ECs are categorized as wh-trace, NP-trace, pro, and PRO under different syntactic constraints with complementary distributions. However, whether ECs are postulated only to suffice theoretical needs is intensively debated. Also, some researchers even challenged the filler-gap dependency by associating the filler directly with the subcategorizer. If ECs are more than theoretical assumptions, the behavioral or neural foundations of ECs should be found. Despite the fruitful findings in previous behavioral and neuroimaging studies on filler-gap dependencies, comprehensive research on the neural basis of ECs is lacking in understanding how the brain recognizes and differentiates the ECs and how they are processed via networks of multiple regions. The dissertation thus comprised three fMRI experiments aimed at investigating how the human brain identified and processed ECs, as well as examining the neural underpinnings of syntactic movements.
    Experiment 1 compared Chinese topic constructions and relative clauses to see how the brain responded to gapped structures with different filler-gap orders (or movement directions). The result showed that both constructions elicited activities in the left inferior frontal gyrus (L-IFG) and the left superior/middle temporal gyrus (L-S/MTG). However, it also indicated that these two core regions, both playing a crucial role in movements, were insensitive to the movement direction. The gap-first structure, i.e., relative clauses, additionally triggered activation in the left anterior temporal lobe (L-aTL). It is hypothesized that the L-aTL was responsible for exploring the semantic content of the verb to predict the categorical and semantic information of the gap and the filler to merge the verb and the gap for a VP temporarily.
    Experiment 2 compared topic and passive constructions in Chinese to see if the brain processed gaps contrastively in various gapped structures. The passive construction in Chinese was analyzed differently from the traditional view of A-movement, where the (covert) null operator movement was proposed for the long passives. In this study, however, we found that the brain activity pattern for passive constructions resembled that for topic constructions, where the co-activation of the L-IFG and the L-MTG did not support the covert movement and the non-movement (complementation) analyses. However, the similarities also failed to either postulate an NP-trace in the gap position or distinguish an NP-trace from a wh-trace (in the topic construction).
    Experiment 3 investigated the neural bases of the two types of non-gapped ECs (pro and PRO) in embedded clauses selected by Chinese hope-verbs (with a pro) and try-verbs (with a PRO), respectively. The two ECs were argued to be distributed complementarily due to their syntactic properties. The results revealed that, while pro elicited the anterior portion of the L-IFG (pars orbitalis, POrb) and the L-MTG, the PRO elicited no significant brain regions, which was an unexpected finding. The distinct brain activation results also indicated the differences in brain activation and processing difficulties. The implication of the POrb may be accounted for by the fact that the construction with a pro did not involve movements, which required a filler to be stored with extra working memory. However, the retrieval of the antecedent was still necessary.
    The activation of the right prefrontal cortex (RPFC) for the passive construction and the construction with a hope-verb (with a pro) was an unexpected finding because the R-PFC was generally assumed for more domain-general or non-language-specific functions. By careful comparison, it was found that the two constructions contained a position allowing a gap or an NP (and sometimes an overt pronoun). Interestingly, the most natural interpretations of the three were distinct. It was also argued that the activation of the R-PFC was an extension of its decision-making function to (1) identify the type of noun and (2) determine the most natural interpretation of its referent.
    The results of the three experiments partially supported the EC framework for their similarities in neural activities. However, the results failed to support the four-way distinction because, at least, the NP-trace and the wh-trace cannot be differentiated. Also, the weak results for PRO impeded us from understanding how the brain processed it.
    Overall, it is argued that the main function of the L-IFG was storing and retrieving necessary information (including the filler) for the gap, while the L-S/MTG was mainly responsible for filler/gap and verb/gap integration. The resembling results also led to the main argument of this dissertation that, in addition to emptiness, the core characteristic of the empty categories is dependency. However, processing the dependency of ECs requires the collaboration of the L-IFG and the L-S/MTG, which also allows us to explain the processing of constructions involving other dependencies, including pronouns, reflexive binding, and subject-verb agreement.

    中文摘要 i Abstract iii Acknowledgement vi List of Tables xi List of Figures xi Chapter 1. Introduction 1 1.1 Empty Categories and the Challenges 2 1.2 The Behavioral Basis of ECs 6 1.2.1 Evidence for ECs 6 1.2.2 Filler-Gap Dependencies and the SRC/ORC Advantage 8 1.3 The Neurophysiological Bases of ECs 13 1.3.1 Evidence for Different Types of ECs 13 1.3.2 Filler-Gap Dependencies and the SRC/ORC Advantage 15 1.4 Neural Substrates for Studies on Filler-Gap Dependencies 17 1.4.1 Neuropsychological Studies: The L-IFG 17 1.4.2 Neuroimaging Studies: The L-IFG and Other Brain Regions 18 1.4.3 Filler-Gap Dependencies and the Directionality of Movement 22 1.5 The Present Study 24 1.5.1 Research Questions, Limitations, and Solutions 24 1.5.2 The Organization of the Dissertation and General Findings 27 Chapter 2. Experiment 1: Filler-Gap Orders 29 2. 1 Introduction 29 2.2 Materials and Methods 38 2.2.1 Participants 38 2.2.2 Experimental Design 38 2.2.2.1 Stimuli 38 2.2.2.2 Procedure 41 2.2.2.3 Image acquisition and analysis 42 2.3. Results 43 2.3.1 Behavioral Results 43 2.3.2 fMRI Analysis 43 2.4 Discussion 46 2.5 Conclusion 53 Chapter 3. Experiment 2: Traces and Movements 55 3.1 Introduction 55 3.1.1 Theoretical Views on Traces 55 3.1.2 Neuroimaging Studies on Movements 60 3.1.2.1 ERP Research on Topicalization and Passivization 60 3.1.2.2. fMRI Research on Syntactic Movements 65 3.1.3 Research Purpose, Questions, and Predictions 71 3.2 Research Design 73 3.2.1 Participants 73 3.2.2 Material 74 3.2.3 Procedure 76 3.2.4.1 Whole Brain Analysis 77 3.2.4.2 ROI Analysis 79 3.3 Results 79 3.3.1 Behavioral Results 79 3.3.2 Whole-Brain Analysis Results 79 3.3.3 ROI Analysis Result 82 3.4 Discussion 83 3.4.1 Topicalization 84 3.4.2 Passivization 87 3.4.3 The TOP-PASS Contrast 90 3.4.4 The PASS-TOP Contrast 91 3.5 Conclusion 98 Chapter 4. Experiment 3: PRO and pro 101 4.1 Introduction 101 4.1.1 Theoretical Account for pro and PRO 101 4.1.2 Behavioral Studies on Non-moved Gaps 105 4.1.3 Neuroimaging Studies on PRO and pro 109 4.1.4 Research Purpose, Questions, and Predictions 112 4.2 Research Design 114 4.2.1 Participants 114 4.2.2 Material 115 4.2.3 Procedure 118 4.2.4 Image Acquisition and Analysis 120 4.2.4.1 Whole-Brain Analysis 120 4.2.4.2 ROI Analysis 121 4.3 Results 121 4.3.1 Behavioral Results 121 4.3.2 Whole-Brain Analysis Results 122 4.3.3 ROI Analysis Results 124 4.4 Discussion 125 4.4.1 Big PRO 125 4.4.2 Small pro 128 4.4.2.1 The Function of the Pars Orbitalis (BA 47) 129 4.4.2.2 The Role of the L-S/MTG 132 4.4.2.3 The Comparison of pro and Trace 134 4.4.3 Theoretical Insight 135 4.5 Conclusion 137 Chapter 5. General Discussion 139 5.1 Filler-first and Gap-first Orders 139 5.2 Moved and Non-moved ECs 141 5.2.1 Moved ECs 141 5.2.2 Non-Moved ECs 145 5.3 Empty Categories? Gaps? 149 5.4 Functions of L-IFG in Sentence Comprehension 153 5.5 Conclusion 161 Chapter 6. Conclusion 162 6.1 Common Neural Bases of ECs 163 6.2 Neural Distinctions of ECs 164 6.3 Neural Foundations of Filler-Gap Orders 165 6.4 Limitations and Future Study 165 References 169 Appendix A: Materials for Experiment 1 197 Appendix B: Materials for Experiment 2 205 Appendix C: Materials for Experiment 3 210

    Acheson, D. J., & Hagoort, P. (2013). Stimulating the brain's language network: Syntactic ambiguity resolution after TMS to the inferior frontal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(10), 1664-1677.
    Ackema, P., & Neeleman, A. (2002). Effects of short-term storage in processing rightward movement. In S. Nooteboom, F. Weerman, & F. Wijnen (Eds.), Storage and computation in the language faculty (pp. 219-256). Kluwer Academic Publishers.
    Adelt, A., Burchert, F., Adani, F., & Nicole, S. (2020). What matters in processing German object relative clauses in aphasia–Timing or morphosyntactic cues? Aphasiology, 34(8), 970-998.
    Agnew, Z. K., van de Koot, H., McGettigan, C., & Scott, S. K. (2014). Do sentences with unaccusative verbs involve syntactic movement? Evidence from neuroimaging. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(9), 1035-1045.
    Ansaldo, U., Lai, J., Jia, F., Siok, W. T., Tan, L. H., & Matthews, S. (2015). Neural basis for processing hidden complexity indexed by small and finite clauses in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 33, 118-127.
    Arai, M., & Keller, F. (2013). The use of verb-specific information for prediction in sentence processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(4), 525-560.
    Assaf, M., Calhoun, V. D., Kuzu, C. H., Kraut, M. A., Rivkin, P. R., Hart, J., Jr, & Pearlson, G. D. (2006). Neural correlates of the object-recall process in semantic memory. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 147(2-3), 115-126.
    Atkinson, E., Wagers, M. W., Lidz, J., Phillips, C., & Omaki, A. (2018). Developing incrementality in filler-gap dependency processing. Cognition, 179, 132-149.
    Badan, L. (2007). High and low periphery: A comparison between Italian and Chinese [Doctoral dissertation], University of Padova, Italy.
    Badan, L., & Del Gobbo, F. (2011). On the syntax of topic and focus in Chinese. In P. Beninca & N. Munaro (Eds.), Mapping the left periphery: The cartography of syntactic structures, Volume 5 (pp. 63-90). Oxford University Press.
    Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2002). Semantic retrieval, mnemonic control, and prefrontal cortex. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1(13), 206-218.
    Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2007). Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 2883-2901.
    Belyk, M., Brown, S., Lim, J., & Kotz, S. A. (2017). Convergence of semantics and emotional expression within the IFG pars orbitalis. NeuroImage, 157(1), 240-248.
    Bemis, D. K., & Pylkkänen, L. (2013). Basic linguistic composition recruits the left anterior temporal lobe and left angular gyrus during both listening and reading. Cerebral Cortex, 23(8), 1859-1873.
    Ben-Shachar, M., Hendler, T., Kahn, I., Ben-Bashat, D., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2003). The neural reality of syntactic transformations: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychological Science, 14(5), 433-440.
    Ben-Shachar, M., Palti, D., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2004). Neural correlates of syntactic movement: converging evidence from two fMRI experiments. NeuroImage, 21(4), 1320-1336.
    Berndt, R. S., Mitchum, C. C., & Wayland, S. (1997). Patterns of sentence comprehension in aphasia: A consideration of three hypotheses. Brain and Language, 60(2), 197-221.
    Betancort, M., Meseguer, E., & Carreiras, M. (2004). The empty category PRO: Processing what cannot be seen. In M. Carreiras & C. Clifton, Jr. (Eds.), The on-line study of sentence comprehension: Eyetracking, ERP and beyond (pp. 95-118). Psychology Press.
    Bever, T. G., & McElree, B. (1988). Empty categories access antecedents during comprehension. Linguistic Inquiry, 19(1), 35-43.
    Bever, T. G., & Sanz, M. (1997). Empty categories access their antecedents during comprehension: Unaccusatives in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 28(1), 69-91.
    Bever, T. G., Straub, K., Shenkman, K., Kim, J. J., & Carrithers, C. (1990). The psychological reality of NP-trace. Proceedings of North East Linguistics Society 20, 1, 1-15.
    Biberauer, T., Holmberg, A., Roberts, I., & Sheenan, M. (2010). Parametric variation: Null subjects in minimalist theory. Cambridge University Press.
    Binder, J., Desai, R., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Scrambling effects on the processing of Japanese sentences: An fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2767-2796.
    Bornkessel, I., Zysset, S., Friederici, A. D., von Cramon, D. Y., & Schlesewsky, M. (2005). Who did what to whom? The neural basis of argument hierarchies during language comprehension. NeuroImage, 26(1), 221-233.
    Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. D., & Schlesewsky, M. (2013). Reconciling time, space and function: A new dorsal–ventral stream model of sentence comprehension. Brain and Language, 125(1), 60-76.
    Bozic, M., Fonteneau, E., Su, L., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2015). Grammatical analysis as a distributed neurobiological function. Human Brain Mapping, 36(3), 1190-1201.
    Brennan, J., Nir, Y., Hasson, U., Malach, R., Heeger, D. J., & Pylkkänen, L. (2012). Syntactic structure building in the anterior temporal lobe during natural story listening. Brain and Language, 120(2), 163-173.
    Buchweitz, A., Mason, R. A., Tomitch, L. M. B., & Just, M. A. (2009). Brain activation for reading and listening comprehension: An fMRI study of modality effects and individual differences in language comprehension. Psychology & Neuroscience, 2(2), 111-123.
    Bulut, T. (2018). Exploration of the neural correlates of syntactic processing in Chinese using functional magnetic resonance imaging and event-related potentials. [Doctoral dissertation], National Central University, Taoyuan, Taiwan.
    Burzio, L. (1986). Italian syntax: A government-binding approach. Springer Dordrecht.
    Caplan, D. (2007). Functional neuroimaging studies of syntactic processing in sentence comprehension: A critical selective review. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(1-2), 32-47.
    Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Waters, G. (1998). Effects of syntactic structure and propositional number on patterns of regional cerebral blood flow. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(4), 541-552.
    Caplan, D., Vijayan, S., Kuperberg, G., West, C., Waters, G., Greve, D., & Dale, A. M. (2001). Vascular responses to syntactic processing: Event-related fMRI study of relative clauses. Human Brain Mapping, 15(1), 26-38.
    Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S. (1999). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 77-126.
    Carreiras, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., Vergara, M., de la Cruz-Pavía, I., & Laka, I. (2010). Subject relative clauses are not universally easier to process: Evidence from Basque. Cognition, 115(1), 79-92.
    Casey, B. J., Forman, S. D., Franzen, P., Berkowitz, A., Braver, T. S., Nystrom, L. E., Thomas, K. M., & Noll, D. C. (2001). Sensitivity of prefrontal cortex to changes in target probability: A functional MRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 13(1), 26-33.
    Chiang, Y.-C. (2017). A movement analysis of right dislocation: The case of Mandarin Chinese. In L. Zhang (Ed.), Proceedings of the 29th North American conference on Chinese Linguistics (NACCL-29) (Vol. 2, pp. 304-315). University of Memphis, TN.
    Cho, S., Metcalfe, A. W. S., Young, C. B., Ryali, S., Geary, D. C., & Menon, V. (2012). Hippocampal-prefrontal engagement and dynamic causal interactions in the maturation of children's fact retrieval. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(9), 1849-1866. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00246
    Cho, S., & Thompson, C. K. (2010). What goes wrong during passive sentence production in agrammatic aphasia: An eyetracking study. Aphasiology, 24(12), 1576-1592.
    Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. MIT Press.
    Chomsky, N. (1973). Conditions on transformation. In S. Anderson & P. Kiparsky (Eds.), A festschrift for Morris Halle (pp. 232-286). Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
    Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding: The Pisa lectures Mouton de Gruyter.
    Chomsky, N. (1982). Some concepts and consequences of the theory of government and binding. MIT Press.
    Chomsky, N. (1986a). Knowledge of Language, its Nature, Origin, and Use. Praeger.
    Chomsky, N. (1993). A minimalist program for linguistic theory. In K. Hale & S. J. Keyser (Eds.), The view from building 20: Essays in linguistics in honor of Sylvain Bromberger (pp. 1-52). MIT Press.
    Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. MIT Press.
    Chomsky, N. (2000). Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In R. Martin, D. Michaels, J. Uriagereka, & K. S. Jay (Eds.), Step by step: Essays on minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik. MIT Press.
    Chomsky, N. (2001). Derivation by phase. In M. Kenstowicz & K. Hale (Eds.), A life in language (pp. 1-52). MIT Press.
    Chomsky, N., & Lasnik, H. (1993). The theory of principles and parameters. In J. Jacobs, A. von Stechow, W. Sternefeld, & T. Vennemann (Eds.), Syntax: An international handbook of contemporary research (pp. 506-569). Walter de Gruyter.
    Chou, T.-L., Booth, J. R., Bitan, T., Burman, D. D., Bigio, J. D., Cone, N. E., Lu, D., & Cao, F. (2006). Developmental and skill effects on the neural correlates of semantic processing to visually presented words. Human Brain Mapping, 27(11), 915-924.
    Constable, R. T., Pugh, K. R., Berroya, E., Mencl, W. E., Westerveld, M., Ni, W., & Shankweiler, D. (2004). Sentence complexity and input modality effects in sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 22(1), 11-21.
    Corver, N., & Nunes, J. (2007). From trace theory to copy theory. In N. Corver & J. Nunes (Eds.), The copy theory of movement (pp. 1-10). John Benjamins Publishing Company.
    Costa, A., Matos, G., & Luegi, P. (2021). Using eye-tracking to study anaphoric relations processing in European Portuguese. Journal of Eyetracking, Visual Cognition and Emotion, 1(1), 50-58.
    Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33(1-2), 25-62.
    Dapretto, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (1999). Form and content: Dissociating syntax and semantics in sentence comprehension. Neuron, 24(2), 427-432.
    de Vincenzi, M. (1991). Syntactic parsing strategies in Italian: The minimal chain principle (Vol. 12). Springer Netherlands.
    Delvin, J. T., Matthews, P. M., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2003). Semantic processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: A combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 71-84.
    Demestre, J., & García-Albea, J. E. (2007). ERP evidence for the rapid assignment of an (appropriate) antecedent to PRO. Cognitive Science, 31(2), 343-354.
    Demestre, J., Meltzer, S., García-Albea, J. E., & Vigil, A. (1999). Identifying the null subject: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Pyscholinguistic Research, 28, 293-312.
    den Ouden, D.-B., Saur, D., Mader, W., Schelter, B., Lukic, S., Wali, E., Timmer, J., & Thompson, C. K. (2012). Network modulation during complex syntactic processing. NeuroImage, 59(1), 815-823.
    Dong, Z., Rohodes, R., & Hestvik, A. (2021). Active gap filling and island constraint in processing the Mandarin “gap-type” topic structure. Frontiers in Communication, 6, 650659.
    Dronkers, N. F., Wilkins, D. P., van Valin Jr., R. D., Redfern, B., & Jaeger, J. J. (2004). Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition, 92(1-2), 145-177.
    Dryer, M. (1992). The Greenbergian word order correlations. Language, 68, 81-138.
    Europa, E., Gitelman, D. R., Kiran, S., & Thompson, C. K. (2019). Neural connectivity in syntactic movement processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 27.
    Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., & D'Ausilio, A. (2009). Broca’s area in language, action, and music. The Neurosciences and Music III-Disorders and Plasticity, 1169(1), 448-458.
    Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., & Roy, A. (2006). Broca’s area: A speech area? In Y. Grodzinsky & K. Amunts (Eds.), Broca's region (pp. 137-152).
    Featherston, S., Gross, M., Münte, T. F., & Clahsen, H. (2000). Brain potentials in the processing of complex sentences: An ERP study of control and raising constructions. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 141-154.
    Featherston, S. (2001). Empty categories in sentence processing. John Benjamins.
    Fecteau, S., Knoch, D., Fregni, F., Sultani, N., Boggio, P., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2007). Diminishing risk-taking behavior by modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex: A direct current stimulation study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(46), 12500-12505.
    Fecteau, S., Knoch, D., Fregni, F., Sultani, N., Boggio, P., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2007). Diminishing risk-taking behavior by modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex: A direct current stimulation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(46), 12500-12505.
    Felser, C., Clahsen, H., & Münte, T. F. (2003). Storage and integration in the processing of filler-gap dependencies: An ERP study of topicalization and wh-movement in German. Brain and Language, 87, 345-354.
    Feng, S., Legaut, J., Yang, L., Zhu, J., Shao, K., & Yang, Y. (2015). Differences in grammatical processing strategies for active and passive sentences: An fMRI study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 33, 104-117.
    Ferstl, E. C., Neumann, J., Bogler, C., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2008). The extended language network: A meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 29(5), 581-593.
    Ferstl, E. C., & Siebörger, F. (2007). Neuroimaging studies of coherence processes. In M. Schwarz-Friese, M. Consten, & M. Knees (Eds.), Anaphors in text: Cognitive, formal and applied approaches to anaphoric reference (pp. 225-240). John Benjamins.
    Ferstl, E. C., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). What does the frontomedian cortex contribute to language processing: Coherence or theory of mind? NeuroImage, 17(3), 1599-1612.
    Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. (2001). Syntactic Working Memory and the Establishment of Filler-Gap Dependencies: Insights from ERPs and fMRI. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 321-338.
    Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., Lohmann, G., von Cramon, D. Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Revisiting the role of Broca's area in sentence processing: Syntactic integration versus syntactic working memory. Human Brain Mapping, 24(2), 79-91.
    Fodor, J. D. (1978). Parsing Strategies and Constraints on Transformations. Linguistic Inquiry, 9(3), 427-473.
    Ford, M. (1983). A method for obtaining measures of local parsing complexity throughout sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22(2), 203-218.
    Frauenfelder, U., Segui, J., & Mehler, J. (1980). Monitoring around the relative clause. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(3), 328-337.
    Frazier, L. (1987). Syntactic processing: Evidence from Dutch. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 5(4), 519-559.
    Frazier, L., & Clifton, C., Jr. (1989). Successive cyclicity in the grammar and the parser. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4(2), 93-126.
    Frazier, L., & Fodor, J. D. (1978). The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition, 6(4), 291-325.
    Frazier, L., & Rayner, K. (1982). Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 14(2), 178-210.
    Friederici, A. D. (2011). The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Psychological Reviews, 91(4), 1357-1392.
    Friederici, A. D. (2012). The cortical language circuit: From auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(5), 262-268.
    Friederici, A. D. (2015). White-matter pathways for speech and language processing. In G. G. Celesia & G. Hickok (Eds.), The Human Auditory System: Fundamental Organization and Clinical Disorders (Vol. 129, pp. 177-186). Elsevier.
    Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of a uniquely human capacity. The MIT Press.
    Friederici, A. D., Bahlmann, J., Friedrich, R., & Makuuchi, M. (2011). The neural basis of recursion and complex syntactic hierarchy. Biolinguistics, 5(1-2), 87-104.
    Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A., & Mecklinger, A. (1996). Temporal structure of syntactic parsing: early and late event-related brain potential effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(5), 1219-1248.
    Friederici, A. D., Makuuchi, M., & Bahlmann, J. (2009). The role of the posterior superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension. Neuroreport, 20(6), 563-568.
    Friederici, A. D., Mecklinger, A., Spencer, K. M., Steinhauer, K., & Donchin, E. (2001). Syntactic parsing preferences and their on-line revisions: a spatio-temporal analysis of event-related brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 11(2), 305-323.
    Friederici, A. D., Opitz, B., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). Segregating semantic and syntactic aspects of processing in the human brain: An fMRI investigation of different word types. Cerebral Cortex, 10(7), 698-705.
    Friederici, A. D., Rüschemeyer, S.-A., Hahne, A., & Fiebach, C. J. (2003). The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: Localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cerebral Cortex, 13(2), 170-177.
    Friedmann, N., Taranto, G., Shapiro, L. P., & Swinney, D. (2008). The leaf fell (the Leaf): The online processing of uunaccusatives. Linguistic Inquiry, 39(3), 355-377.
    Gelormini-Lezama, C., & Almor, A. (2011). Repeated names, overt pronouns, and null pronouns in Spanish. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(3), 437-454.
    Gerardin, E., Sirigu, A., Lehéricy, S., Poline, J.-B., Gaymard, B., Marsault, C., Agid, Y., & Le Bihan, D. (2000). Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. Cerebral Cortex, 10(11), 1093-1104.
    Gerlach, C., Law, I., & Paulson, O. B. (2002). When action turns into words: Activation of motor-based knowledge during categorization of manipulable objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1230-1239.
    Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68(1), 1-76.
    Gibson, E., & Wu, H.-H. I. (2013). Processing Chinese relative clauses in context. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(1-2), 125-155.
    Gouvea, A. C. (2003). Processing syntactic complexity: Cross-linguistic differences and ERP evidence [Doctoral dissertation]. University of Maryland, College Park.
    Grimshaw, J. (1990). Argument structure. The MIT Press.
    Grodzinsky, Y. (1986). Language deficits and the theory of syntax. Brain and Language, 27, 135-159.
    Grodzinsky, Y. (1995). A restrictive theory of agrammatic comprehension. Brain and Language, 23(1), 1-71.
    Grodzinsky, Y. (2004). Variation in Broca's region: Preliminary cross-methodological comparisons. In J. Rooryck & P. Pica (Eds.), Variation and Universals in Biolinguistics (pp. 171-193). Elsevier.
    Grodzinsky, Y. (2006). A blueprint for a brain map of syntax. In Y. Grodzinsky & K. Amunts (Eds.), Broca's region. Oxford University Press.
    Grodzinsky, Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Neuroimaging of syntax and syntactic processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 240-246.
    Grodzinsky, Y., & Santi, A. (2004). An fMRI investigation into the reflexive binding vs. movement Cognitive Neuroscience Society, April, San Francisco.
    Grodzinsky, Y., & Santi, A. (2008). The battle for Broca’s region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(12), 474-480.
    Gutierrez, C., Cola, M. G., Seltzer, B., & Cusick, C. (2000). Neurochemical and connectional organization of the dorsal pulvinar complex in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 419(1), 61-86.
    Hammer, A., Jansma, B. M., Tempelmann, C., & Münte, T. F. (2011). Neural mechanisms of anaphoric reference revealed by fMRI. Frontiers in Psychology, 2.
    Hashimoto, M. (1987). Hanyu beidongshi de lishi quyu fazhan [The historical and geographical development of Chinese passive constructions]. Zhongguo Yuwen, 196, 36-49.
    Hawkins, J. A. (1990). A parsing theory of word order universals. Linguistic Inquiry, 21(2), 223-261.
    Hawkins, J. A. (1999). Processing complexity and filler-gap dependencies across grammars. Language, 75(2), 244-285.
    Hebb, A. O., & Ojemann, G. A. (2013). The thalamus and language revisited. Brain and Language, 126(1), 99-108.
    Her, O.-S. (2009). Unifying the long passive and the short passive: On the bei construction in Taiwan Mandarin. Language and linguistics, 10(3), 421-470.
    Hestvik, A., Maxfield, N., Schwartz, R. G., & Shafer, V. (2007). Brain reponses to filled gaps. Brain and Language, 100, 301-316.
    Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393-402.
    Hirotani, M., Makuuchi, M., Rüschemeyer, S.-A., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Who was the agent? The neural correlates of reanalysis processes during sentence comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 32(11), 1775-1787.
    Hoen, M., Deprez, V., & Dominey, P. F. (2007). Do you agree? Electrophysiological characterization of online agreement checking during the comprehension of correct French passive sentences. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(5), 395-421.
    Holmes, V. M., & O'Regan, K. J. (1981). Eye fixation patterns during the reading of relative-clause sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20(4), 417-430.
    Horn, G. M. (2011). Essentials of functional grammar: A structure-neutral theory of movement, control, and anaphor. Mouton de Gruyter.
    Hornstein, N. (1999). Movement and control. Linguistic Inquiry, 30(1), 69-96.
    Hsiao, F., & Gibson, E. (2003). Processing relative clauses in Chinese. Cognition, 90(1), 3-27.
    Hsu, C.-C. N., & Bruening, B. (2003). Investigating gap-filler dependencies in Chinese: Is there an ‘active gap’? 9th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing, Glasgow, Scotland.
    Huang, C.-T. J. (1982). Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of Grammar [Doctoral dissertation], MIT, Cambridge, Massachusetts.
    Huang, C.-T. J. (1989). Pro-drop in Chinese: A generalized control theory. In O. Jaeggli & K. J. Safier (Eds.), The null subject parameter. Kluwer Academic Publishers.
    Huang, C.-T. J. (1999). Chinese passives in comparative perspective. Tsing Hua Journal of Chinese Studies, 24(4), 423-509.
    Huang, C.-T. J., Li, Y.-H. A., & Li, Y. (2009). The syntax of Chinese. Cambridge University Press.
    Huang, R.-H. (2023). The syntax of Mandarin long gei passives revisited. Studies in Chinese Linguistics, 44(1), 51-73.
    Huang, Y.-C., & Kaiser, E. (2008). Investigating filler-gap dependencies in Chinese topicalization. In M. K. M. Chan & H. Kang (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20th North American Conference on Chinese Linguistics (NACCL-20) (Vol. 2, pp. 927-941). The Ohio State University.
    Humphries, C., Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., & Liebenthal, E. (2006). Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(4), 665-679.
    Jackson, C. N., Lorimor, H., & van Hell, J. G. (2020). The ERP correlates of thematic role assignment for passive versus active sentences. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 54, 100886.
    Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F., & Thulborn, K. R. (1996). Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, 274(5284), 114-116.
    Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P. J. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(2), 159-201.
    Kielar, A., Milman, L., Bonakdarpour, B., & Thompson, C. K. (2011). Neural correlates of covert and overt production of tense and agreement morphology: Evidence from fMRI. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24(2), 183-201.
    Kim, J., Koizumi, M., Ikuta, N., Fukumitsu, Y., Kimura, N., Iwata, K., Watanabe, J., Yokoyama, S., Sato, S., Horie, K., & Kawashima, R. (2009). Scrambling effects on the processing of Japanese sentences: An fMRI study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22(2), 151-166.
    King, J. W., & Just, M. A. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(5), 580-602.
    King, J. W., & Kutas, M. (1995). Who did what and when? Using word- and clause-level EPRs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(3), 376-395.
    Kinno, R., Kawamura, M., Shioda, S., & Sakai, K. L. (2008). Neural correlates of noncanonical syntactic processing revealed by a picture-sentence matching task. Human Brain Mapping, 29(9), 1015-1027.
    Kluender, R., & Kutas, M. (1993). Bridging the gap: Evidence from ERPs on the processing of unbounded dependencies Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(2), 196-214.
    Kluender, R., & Münte, T. F. (1998). Subject/object asymmetries: ERPs to grammatical and ungrammatical wh-questions. In Poster presented at the 11th annual CUNY conference on human sentence processing, Rutgers University.
    Knoch, D., Pascual-Leone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V., & Fehr, E. (2006). Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science, 314(5800), 829-832.
    Knoch, D., Pascual-Leone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V., & Fehr, E. (2006). Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science, 314(5800), 829-832.
    Krain, A. L., Wilson, A. M., Arbuckle, R., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2006). Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: A meta-analysis of decision-making. NeuroImage, 32(1), 477-484.
    Krain, A. L., Wilson, A. M., Arbuckle, R., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2006). Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: A meta-analysis of decision-making. NeuroImage, 32(1), 477-484.
    Kural, M. (1997). Postverbal constituents in Turkish and the linear correspondence axiom. Linguistic Inquiry, 28(3), 498-519.
    Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203-205.
    Kwon, N., Kluender, R., Kutas, M., & Polinsky, M. (2013). Subject/object processing asymmetries in Korean relative clauses: Evidence from ERP data. Language (Baltim), 89(3), 537-585.
    Landau, I. (2003). Movement out of control. Linguistic Inquiry, 34(3), 471-498.
    Landau, I. (2006). Severing the distribution of PRO from case. Syntax, 9(2), 153-170.
    Lau, E., Stroud, C., Plesch, S., & Phillips, C. (2006). The role of structural prediction in rapid syntactic analysis. Brain and Language, 98(1), 74-88.
    Law, S.-P. (2000). Structural prominence hypothesis and Chinese aphasic sentence comprehension. Brain and Language, 74(2), 260-268.
    Lee, T. W., & Chan, S. H. (2023). Better early than late for a filler: An fMRI study on the filler-gap order in language. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 66, 101126.
    Leiken, K., McElree, B., & Liina, P. (2015). Filling predictable and unpredictable gaps, with and without similarity-based interference: evidence for LIFG effects of dependency processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 6.
    Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (1995). Unaccusativity: At the syntax-lexical semantics interface. The MIT Press.
    Li, J., Fabre, M., Luh, W.-M., & Hale, J. (2018). The role of syntax during pronoun resolution: Evidence from fMRI. Proceedings of the Eight Workshop on Cognitive Aspects of Computational Language Learning and Processing, 56-64.
    Li, J., Wang, S., Luh, W.-M., Pylkkänen, L., & Yang, Y. (2020). Cortical processing of reference in language revealed by computational models. bioRxiv, 11, 24.
    Lin, C.-J. (2006). Chinese is no exception: Universal subject preference of relative clause processing. 19th annual CUNY conference on human sentence processing, New York, NY.
    Lin, C.-J, & Bever, T. G. (2006). Subject preference in the processing of relative clauses in Chinese. Proceedings of the 25th west coast conference on formal linguistics, 25.
    Lin, C.-J., & Bever, T. G. (2011). Garden path and the comprehension of head-final relative clauses. In H. Yamashita, Y. Hirose, & J. L. Packard (Eds.), Processing and Producing Head-final Structures (pp. 277-298). Springer.
    Linzen, T., Marantz, A., & Pylkkänen, L. (2013). Syntactic context effects in visual word recognition. The Mental Lexicon, 8(2), 117-139.
    Liu, B., Wang, Z., & Jin, Z. (2010). The effects of punctuations in Chinese sentence comprehension: An ERP study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23(1), 66-80.
    Liu, H., Jiang, Y., Zhang, B., Ma, L., He, S., & Weng, X. (2013). The orthographic sensitivity to written Chinese in the occipital‑temporal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 227, 387-396.
    Liu, T., & Jiang, H. (2016). Neural mechanisms of syntactic movement: An ERPs study of Chinese passive sentences. Linguistic Sciences, 15(6), 612-624.
    Lu, F., Yang, W., Wei, D., Sun, J., Zhang, Q., & Qiu, J. (2022). Superior frontal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus connectivity mediates the relationship between neuroticism and thought suppression. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 16(3), 1400-1409.
    MacDonald, M. C. (1989). Priming effects from gaps to antecedents. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4(1), 35-56.
    Mack, J. E., Meltzer-Asscher, A., Barbieri, E., & Thompson, C. K. (2013). Neural correlates of processing passive sentences. Brain Sciences, 3(3), 1198-1214.
    Macleod, A. K., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M., Petersen, S. E., & Raichle, M. E. (1998). Right anterior prefrontal cortex activation during semantic monitoring and working memory. NeuroImage, 7(1), 41-48.
    Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2006). Animacy in processing relative clauses: The hikers that rocks crush. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(4), 466-490.
    Makuuchi, M., Bahlmann, J., Anwander, A., & Friederici, A. D. (2009). Segregating the core computational faculty of human language from working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(20), 8362-8367.
    Makuuchi, M., Grodzinsky, Y., Amunts, K., Santi, A., & Friederici, A. D. (2013). Processing noncanonical sentences in Broca’s region: Reflections of movement distance and type. Cerebral Cortex, 23(3), 694-702.
    Manzini, M. R. (1983). On control and control theory. Linguistic Inquiry, 14(3), 421-446.
    Martin, A. (2007). The Representation of Object Concepts in the Brain. Annual Review of Pyshology, 58, 25-45.
    Matchin, W., Hammerly, C., & Lau, E. (2017). The role of the IFG and pSTS in syntactic prediction: Evidence from a parametric study of hierarchical structure in fMRI. Cortex, 88, 106-123.
    Matchin, W., & Hickok, G. (2020). The cortical organization of syntax. Cerebral Cortex, 30(3), 1481-1498.
    Matchin, W., Sprouse, J., & Hickok, G. (2014). A structural distance effect for backward anaphora in Broca’s area: an fMRI study. Brain and Language, 138, 1-11.
    McElree, B., & Bever, T. G. (1989). The psychological reality of linguistically defined gaps. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18(1), 21-35.
    McMillan, C. T., Clark, R., Gunawardena, D., Ryant, N., & Grossman, M. (2012). fMRI evidence for strategic decision-making during resolution of pronoun reference. Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 674-687.
    Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., Steinhauer, K., & Friederici, A. D. (1995). Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related potentials. Memory & Cognition, 23(4), 477-494.
    Meltzer, J. A., McArdle, J. J., Schafer, R. J., & Braun, A. R. (2010). Neural aspects of sentence comprehension: Syntactic complexity, reversibility, and reanalysis. Cerebral Cortex, 20(8), 1853-18664.
    Meltzer-Asscher, A., Mack, J. E., Barbieri, E., & Thompson, C. K. (2015). How the brain processes different dimensions of argument structure complexity: Evidence from fMRI. Brain and Language, 142, 65-75.
    Meyer, K., & Damasio, A. R. (2009). Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(7), 376-382.
    Miyamoto, E. T., & Nakamura, M. (2003). Subject/object asymmetries in the processing of relative clauses in Japanese. In G. Garding & M. Tsujimura (Eds.), WCCFL 22 Proceedings (pp. 342-355). Cascadilla Press.
    Müller, R.-A., & Basho, S. (2004). Are nonlinguistic functions in “Broca's area” prerequisites for language acquisition? FMRI findings from an ontogenetic viewpoint. Brain and Language, 89(2), 329-336.
    Mushiake, H., Sakamoto, K., Saito, N., Inui, T., Aihara, K., & Tanji, J. (2009). Chapter 1 Involvement of the Prefrontal Cortex in Problem Solving. International Review of Neurobiology, 85, 1-11.
    Mushiake, H., Sakamoto, K., Saito, N., Inui, T., Aihara, K., & Tanji, J. (2009). Involvement of the prefrontal cortex in problem solving. International Review of Neurobiology, 85, 1-11.
    Nakano, Y., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2002). Antecedent priming at trace positions in Japanese long-distance scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 531-571.
    Newman, S. D., Ikuta, N., & Burns, T., Jr. (2010). The effect of semantic relatedness on syntactic analysis: An fMRI study. Brain and Language, 113(2), 51-58.
    Ng, S. (2008). An Active Gap Strategy in the Processing of Filler-Gap Dependencies in Chinese. The 20th North American Conference on Chinese Linguistics,
    Ng, S. (2009). Processing Chinese Empty Categories The City University of New York].
    Nieuwland, M. S., Petersson, K. M., & van Berkum, J. J. A. (2007). On sense and reference: Examining the functional neuroanatomy of referential processing. NeuroImage, 37(3), 993-1004.
    Novais-Santos, S., Gee, J., Shah, M., Troiani, V., Work, M., & Grossman, M. (2007). Resolving sentence ambiguity with planning and working memory resources: Evidence from fMRI. NeuroImage, 37(1), 361-378.
    O'Grady, W. (1997). Syntactic Development. University of Chicago Press.
    Ohta, S., Koizumi, M., & Sakai, K. L. (2017). Dissociating effects of scrambling and topicalization within the left frontal and temporal language areas: An fMRI study in Kaqchikel Maya. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 748.
    Omaki, A., Lau, E. F., White, I. D., Dakan, M. L., Aaron, A., & Phillips, C. (2015). Hyper-active gap filling. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 384.
    Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P. J., & Swinney, D. A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 786-803.
    Osterhout, L., & Swinney, D. A. (1993). On the temporal course of gap-filling during comprehension of verbal passives. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22(2), 273-286.
    Packard, J. L., Ye, Z., & Zhou, X. (2011). Filler-Gap Processing in Mandarin Relative Clauses: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials. In H. Yamashita, Y. Hirose, & J. L. Packard (Eds.), Processing and Producing Head-final Structures (pp. 219-240). Springer.
    Park, M.-K., Cho, E., & Chung, W. (2015). Backward antecedent search of the null subject pronominal (pro) in Korean: An ERP study. Language Research, 51(3), 537-568.
    Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Natural Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976-987.
    Perlmutter, D. M. (1978). Impersonal passives and the unaccusative hypothesis. Proceedings of the 4th annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistic Society, 4, 157-189.
    Pesetsky, D. M. (1982). Paths and categories [Master's thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
    Philipp, M., Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. D., Bisang, W., & Schlesewsky, M. (2008). The role of animacy in the real time comprehension of Mandarin Chinese: Evidence from auditory event-related brain potentials. Brain and Language, 105(2), 112-133.
    Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Abada, S. H. (2005). ERP effects of the processing of syntactic long-distance dependencies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(3), 407-428.
    Pickering, M. J., & Barry, G. (1991). Sentence processing without empty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes, 6(3), 229-259.
    Pollard, C., & Sag, I. A. (1994). Head-driven phrase structure grammar. University of Chicago Press.
    Pulvermüller, F., Huss, M., Kherif, F., Moscoso del Prado Martin, F., Hauk, O., & Shtyrov, Y. (2006). Motor cortex maps articulatory features of speech sounds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(20), 7865-7870.
    Pylkkänen, L., Oliveri, B., & Smart, A. J. (2009). Semantics vs. world knowledge in prefrontal cortex. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(9), 1313-1334.
    Ragland, D. J., Turetsky, B. I., Gur, R. C., Gunning-Dixon, F., Turner, T., Schroeder, L., Chan, R., & Gur, R. E. (2002). Working memory for complex figures: An fMRI comparison of letter and fractal n-back tasks. Neuropsychology, 16(3), 370-379.
    Ranganath, C., & D'Esposito, M. (2005). Directing the mind's eye: Prefrontal, inferior and medial temporal mechanisms for visual working memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 175-182.
    Rochemont, M. (1992). Bounding rightward Ā-dependencies. In H. Goodluck & M. Rochemont (Eds.), Island constraints: Theory, acquisition and processing (pp. 373-397). Springer, Dordrecht.
    Rodd, J. M., Vitello, S., Woollams, A. M., & Patti, A. (2015). Localising semantic and syntactic processing in spoken and written language comprehension: An Activation Likelihood Estimation meta-analysis. Brain and Language, 141, 89-102.
    Rogalsky, C., & Hickok, G. (2009). Selective attention to semantic and syntactic features modulates sentence processing networks in anterior temporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 19(4), 786-796.
    Rogalsky, C., & Hickok, G. (2011). The role of Broca's area in sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1664-1680.
    Rogalsky, C., Matchin, W., & Hickok, G. (2008). Brocas area, sentence comprehension, and working memory: An fMRI study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, 1-13.
    Rogers, T. T., Hocking, J., Noppeney, U., Mechelli, A., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Patterson, K., & Price, C. J. (2006). Anterior temporal cortex and semantic memory: Reconciling findings from neuropsychology and functional imaging. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(3), 201-213.
    Ross, J. R. (1967). Constraints on variables in syntax [Doctoral dissertation], Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
    Santi, A., Friederici, A. D., Makuuchi, M., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2010). An fMRI study dissociating distance measures computed by Broca’s area in movement processing: clause boundary vs. identity. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 654. https://doi.org/doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00654
    Santi, A., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2007). Working memory and syntax interact in Broca's area. NeuroImage, 37(1), 8-17.
    Santi, A., & Grodzinsky, Y. (2012). Broca’s area and sentence comprehension: A relationship parasitic on dependency, displacement or predictability? Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 821-832.
    Scheibe, C., Ullsperger, M., Sommer, W., & Heekeren, H., R. (2010). Effects of parametrical and trial-to-trial variation in prior probability processing revealed by simultaneous electroencephalogram / functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(49), 16709-16717.
    Scheibe, C., Ullsperger, M., Sommer, W., & Heekeren, H., R. (2010). Effects of parametrical and trial-to-trial variation in prior probability processing revealed by simultaneous electroencephalogram/functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(49), 16709-16717.
    Schriefers, H., Friederici, A. D., & Kuhn, K. (1995). The processing of locally ambiguous relative clauses in German. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(4), 499-520.
    Schulze, K., & Koelsch, S. (2012). Working memory for speech and music. The Neurosciences and Music IV-Learning and Memory, 1252(1), 229-236.
    Scott, S. K., Blank, C. C., Rosen, S., & Wise, R. J. S. (2000). Identification of a pathway for intelligible speech in the left temporal lobe. Brain, 123(12), 2400-2406.
    Segaert, K., Kempen, G., Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2013). Syntactic priming and the lexical boost effect during sentence production and sentence comprehension: An fMRI study. Brain and Language, 124(2), 174-183.
    Shetreet, E., & Friedmann, N. (2012). Stretched, jumped, and fell: An fMRI investigation of reflexive verbs and other intransitives. NeuroImage, 60(3), 1800-1806.
    Shetreet, E., & Friedmann, N. (2014). The processing of different syntactic structures: FMRI investigation of the linguistic distinction between wh-movement and verb movement. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 27(1), 1-17.
    Shetreet, E., Friedmann, N., & Hadar, U. (2009). An fMRI study of syntactic layers: Sentential and lexical aspects of embedding. NeuroImage, 48(4), 707-716.
    Shetreet, E., Friedmann, N., & Hadar, U. (2010). The neural correlates of linguistic distinctions: Unaccusative and unergative verbs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(10), 2306-2315.
    Shi, D. (1992). The nature of topic comment constructions and topic chains [Doctoral dissertation], University of Southern California.
    Sigurðsson, H. Á. (2008). The case of PRO. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 26, 403-450.
    Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 1657-1661.
    Snijders, T. M., Vosse, T., Kempen, G., van Berkum, J. J. A., Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2009). Retrieval and unification of syntactic structure in sentence comprehension: An fMRI study using word-category ambiguity. Cerebral Cortex, 19(7), 1493-1503.
    Staub, A., Clifton, C., Jr., & Frazier, L. (2006). Heavy NP shift is the parser’s last resort: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(3), 389-406.
    Steinhauer, K. (2003). Electrophysiological correlates of prosody and punctuation. Brain and Language, 86(1), 142-164.
    Stowe, L. A. (1986). Parsing WH-constructions: Evidence for online gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 227-245.
    Stowe, L. A., & Carlson, G. N. (1991). Filling gaps on-line: Use of lexical and semantic information in sentence processing. Language and Speech, 34(4), 319-340.
    Stowe, L. A., Broere, C. A. J., Paans, A. M. J., Wijers, A. A., Mulder, G., Vaalburg, W., & Zwarts, F. (1998). Localizing components of a complex task: Sentence processing and working memory. Neuroreport, 9(13), 2995-2999.
    Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Rauch, S. (1996). Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain and Language, 52(3), 452-473.
    Stroud, C., & Phillips, C. (2012). Examining the evidence for an independent semantic analyzer: An ERP study in Spanish. Brain and Language, 120(2), 108-126.
    Sun, Z., Shi, Y., Guo, P., Yang, Y., & Zhu, Z. (2021). Independent syntactic representation identified in left front-temporal cortex during Chinese sentence comprehension. Brain and Language, 214, 104907.
    Sussman, R. S., & Sedivy, J. C. (2003). The time-course of processing syntactic dependencies: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(2), 143-163.
    Tang, S.-W. (2001). A complementation approach to Chinese passives and its consequences. Linguistics, 39(2), 257-295.
    Tesink, C., Petersson, K. M., van Berkum, J. J. A., van den Brink, D., Buitelaar, J. K., & Hagoort, P. (2009). Unification of speaker and meaning in language comprehension: An fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), 2085-2099.
    Thompson, C. K., Bonakdarpour, B., Fix, S. C., Blumenfeld, H. K., Parrish, T. B., Gitelman, D. R., & Mesulam, M. M. (2007). Neural correlates of verb argument structure processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(11), 1753-1767.
    Traxler, M., Morris, R. K., & Seely, R. E. (2002). Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 69-90.
    Travis, L. (1984). Parameters and effects of word order variation [Doctoral dissertation], MIT.
    Tsai, W.-T. D. (1994). On nominal islands and LF extraction in Chinese. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 12, 121-175.
    Tyler, L. K., Stamatakis, E., Post, B., Randall, B., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2005). Temporal and frontal systems in speech comprehension: An fMRI study of past tense processing. Neuropsychologia, 43(13), 1963-1974.
    Ueno, M., & Garnsey, S. M. (2007). Gap-filling vs. filling gaps: Event-related brain indices of subject and object relative clauses in Japanese. Proceedings of the 15th Japanese/Korean Linguistics Conference, Standford, CA.
    van Dam, W. O., & Desai, R. H. (2016). The semantics of syntax: The grounding of transitive and intransitive constructions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(5), 693-709.
    van der Meulen, I. (2004). Syntactic movement and comprehension deficits in Broca's aphasia. Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics.
    Vandenberghe, R., Nobre, A. C., & Price, C. J. (2002). The response of left temporal cortex to sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 550-560.
    Vasishth, S., Chen, Z., Li, Q., & Guo, G. (2013). Processing Chinese relative clauses: Evidence for the subject-relative advantage. PloS ONE, 8(10), e77006.
    Visser, M., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). Differential contributions of bilateral ventral anterior temporal lobe and left anterior superior temporal gyrus to semantic processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 3121-3131.
    Vogelzang, M., Foppolo, F., Guasti, M. T., van Rijn, H., & Hendriks, P. (2020). Reasoning about alternative forms is costly: The processing of null and overt pronouns in Italian using pupillary responses. Discourse Processes, 57(2), 158-183.
    Vogelzang, M., Hendriks, P., & van Rijn, H. (2015). Processing overt and null subject pronouns in Italian: A cognitive model 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Pasadena.
    Wagner, A. D., Shannon, B. J., Kahn, I., & Buckner, R. L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 445-453.
    Wang, C. T. (1970). A transformational approach to Chinese ba and bei. [Doctoral dissertation], University of Texas.
    Wang, X., Feng, S., Zhou, T., Want, R., Wu, G., Ni, F., & Yang, Y. (2021). Brain regions involved in underlying syntactic processing of Mandarin Chinese intransitive verbs: An fMRI study. Brain Sciences, 11(8), 983.
    Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H., R, Burchert, F., Heinemann, S., De Bleser, R., & Villrigner, A. (2004). Neural correlates of syntactic transformations. Human Brain Mapping, 22(1), 72-81.
    Westerlund, M., Kastner, I., Kaabi, M. A., & Pylkkänen, L. (2015). The LATL as locus of composition: MEG evidence from English and Arabic. Brain and Language, 141, 124-134.
    Westerlund, M., & Pylkkänen, L. (2014). The role of the left anterior temporal lobe in semantic composition vs. semantic memory. Neuropsychologia, 57, 59-70.
    Williams, E. (1980). Predication. Linguistic Inquiry, 11(1), 203-238.
    Williams, E. (1981). Argument structure and morphology. The Linguistic Review, 1(1), 81-114.
    Williams, E. (1984). Grammatical relations. Linguistic Inquiry, 15(4), 639-673.
    Wilson, S. M., Dronkers, N. F., Ogar, J. M., Jang, J., Growdon, M. E., Agosta, F., Henry, M. L., Miller, B. L., & Gorno-Tempini, M. L. (2010). Neural correlates of syntactic processing in the nonfluent variant of primary progressive aphasia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(50), 16845-16854.
    Wu, D. H., Waller, S., & Chatterjee, A. (2007). The functional neuroanatomy of thematic role and locative relation knowledge. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1542-1555.
    Xiong, Y., Dekydtspotter, L., & Newman, S. (2019). When embeddedness matters: Electrophysiological evidence for the role of head noun position in Chinese relative clause processing. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 51, 236-257.
    Xu, J., Wang, J., Fan, L., Li, H., Zhang, W., Hu, Q., & Jiang, T. (2006). Tractography-based parcellation of the human middle temporal gyrus. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 18883.
    Xu, J., Wang, J., Fan, L., Li, H., Zhang, W., Hu, Q., & Jiang, T. (2016). Tractography-based parcellation of the human middle temporal gyrus. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 18883.
    Xu, K., Duann, J.-R., Hung, D. L., & Wu, D. H. (2019). Preference for object relative clauses in Chinese sentence comprehension: Evidence from online self-paced reading time. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2210.
    Xu, K., Wu, D. H., & Duann, J.-r. (2020). Enhanced left inferior frontal to left superior temporal effective connectivity for complex sentence comprehension: FMRI evidence from Chinese relative clause processing. Brain and Language, 200, 1-10.
    Xu, L. (1986). Free empty category. Linguistic Inquiry, 17(1), 75-93.
    Yang, C. L., Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Wu, J. T. (1999). Comprehension of referring expressions in Chinese. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(5-6), 715-743.
    Yang, Y., & Tao, L. (2014). Neural mechanisms of trace in Chinese topicalized constructions. Social Sciences in China, 35(1), 86-111.
    Ye, Z., & Zhou, X. (2008). Involvement of cognitive control in sentence comprehension: Evidence from ERPs. Brain Research, 1203, 103-115.
    Ye, Z., & Zhou, X. (2009). Conflict control during sentence comprehension: fMRI evidence. NeuroImage, 48, 280-290.
    Yokoyama, S., Watanabe, J., Iwata, K., Ikuta, N., Haji, T., Usui, N., Taira, M., Miyamoto, T., Nakamura, W., Sato, S., Horie, K., & Kawashima, R. (2007). Is Broca’s area involved in the processing of passive sentences? An event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 989-996.
    Zhang, B., He, S., & Weng, X. (2018). Localization and functional characterization of an occipital visual word form sensitive area. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 6723.
    Zhang, S., Li, J., Yang, Y., & Hale, J. (2022). Decoding the silence: Neural bases of zero pronoun resolution in Chinese. Brain and Language, 224, 105050.
    Zurif, E., Swinney, D., Prather, P., Solomon, J., & Bushell, C. (1993). An on-line analysis of syntactic processing in Broca′ s and Wernicke′ s aphasia. Brain and Language, 45(3), 448-464.

    下載圖示
    QR CODE