研究生: |
黃博聖 Huang, Po-Sheng |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
預期一致性、結果價性與自我肯定對正、負向回饋的處理與回憶效果 The influence of Expectancy Violation, Outcome Valence, Self-Affirmation on the processing and memory of positive and negative feedback. |
指導教授: |
陳學志
Chen, Hsueh-Chih |
學位類別: |
博士 Doctor |
系所名稱: |
教育心理與輔導學系 Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling |
論文出版年: | 2013 |
畢業學年度: | 101 |
語文別: | 中文 |
論文頁數: | 143 |
中文關鍵詞: | 預期一致性 、結果價性 、自我肯定 、回饋 、記憶 |
英文關鍵詞: | Expectancy Violation, Outcome Valence, Self-Affirmation, Feedback, Memory |
論文種類: | 學術論文 |
相關次數: | 點閱:138 下載:23 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
人們傾向會去處理或回憶正向或負向回饋?對於不同類型回饋的處理與回憶,是否會受到不同結果狀態所影響,像是好、壞結果,或者是結果與自己預期的一致性?如何讓人們願意去處理可能會對自尊帶來威脅的負向回饋?皆為本研究所關心的研究問題。本研究以四個實驗,來檢驗預期一致性(預期一致或預期違反)、結果價性(好結果或壞結果)與自我肯定(有或無自我肯定)因素,如何影響人們對於正、負向回饋訊息的處理與回憶。實驗一與實驗二分別以心理健康測量及理性思考作業為材料,透過給予參與者在練習階段與正式評定階段的假分數,來操弄預期變項與結果變項,以檢驗預期一致性與結果價性兩因素,是否會對回饋的處理造成影響。實驗三與實驗四,則使用與實驗二相同的實驗材料與操弄方式,但新增自我肯定變項,來檢驗自我肯定程序如何改變預期一致性與結果價性因素對回饋處理的影響(實驗三的自我肯定作業是在理性思考作業前進行;實驗四則是在理性思考作業之結果分數出現後、回饋呈現前進行)。整體結果顯示:(1)參與者對負向回饋有較多的觀看時間或較好的自由回憶(實驗一與實驗三有顯著效果,實驗四為臨界顯著)。(2)結果價性因素會對回饋的處理與回憶產生影響,而預期一致性因素則不會(實驗二、三、四皆獲致此結果)。此可能的解釋為,人們在好結果的情境下,會降低自我保護,對威脅到自我的負向回饋,有較高的處理與回憶效果;然而在壞結果的情境,則會啟動自我保護,降低對負向回饋的處理與回憶。另一方面,預期一致性因素則不會對回饋的處理產生影響。換句話說,人們會受到結果價性因素的影響,而盡可能避免去處理會對自尊帶來威脅的訊息;然而,個體卻不會受到預期一致性因素的影響,不會試圖去尋找結果發生的可能原因。(3)自我肯定因素,會讓獲得壞結果的個體,提升其處理具有威脅性質之負向回饋的意願,並對負向回饋有較好的回憶;不過此自我肯定程序,需在結果分數出現後再進行,才會有效果(實驗四有效果,實驗三則無)。最後,將研究結果與過去文獻進行比較,並探討此結果在教育情境與現實生活中的意涵。
Do people prefer to process and recall positive or negative feedback? Would the processing and memory of different feedback be affected by the different outcome or expectancy situation? How to let people process the negative feedback which could threaten our self-esteem? All of above are the questions our research concerned. Four experiments were conducted to exame the influence of Expectancy Violation (Expectancy Confirmation VS Expectancy Violation), Outcome Valence (Good Outcome VS Bad Outcome) and Self-Affirmation (Self-Affirmation VS non-Self-Affirmation) factors on the processing and memory of Feedback (Positive Feedback VS Negative Feedback). The measurement of Mental Heath (MH) and Rational Thinking Task (RTT) were used as material in Experiment 1 and 2 sequentially, by giving bogus score in the Pratice Phase and Formal Rating Phase to manipulate the variables of Expectancy and Outcome. Experiment 3 and 4 were used the same materials and manipulation as Experiment 2, but adding the variable of Self-Affimation to exame whether it would change the influence of Expectancy Violation and Outcome Valence on Feedback (The Self-Affirmation Task was conducted before the RTT in Experiment 3, but after the outcome score in the RTT and before the presentation of feedback in Experiment 4). Overall, the results show that (1) People would view negative feedback longer and recall better than positive feedback. (2) The factor of Outcome Valence would influence the processing and recall of feedback, but the factor of Expectancy Violation wouldn’t (the result of Experment 2, 3, 4). Specificallly, in Good Outcome situation, people would lessen self-protection, and process or recall more of negative feedback which threatening ourselves. But in Bad Outcome situation, people would activate self-protection, and lower the process or recall of negative feedback. On the other hand, the factor of Expectancy Violation wouldn’t influence the process of feedback. In other words, people would avoid to processs the threatening feedback (which be activated by the factor of Outcome Valence), but would not make the effort to find the possible causes of the outcome (which be activated by the factor of Expectancy Violation). (3) The factor of Self-Affirmaion would enhance the participants of bad outcme situation to process and recall more of negative feedback. However, the effect of Self-Affirmation only existed after the appearance of outcome score (existed in Experiment 4, but not in Experiment 3). Implications of psychological theory and practice were discussed.
林俊宏(2009)。台灣簡式人格量表的制訂:以瑞典大學人格量表簡化修訂。輔仁大學心理學系碩士論文。
張如芬(2006)。諮商員之人格特質、宗教信仰、自我效能與諮商表現之相關研究。花蓮教育大學諮商與輔導研究所碩士論文。
黃靜君(2013)。特質肯定與能力增長觀對怕被笑學生迎接挑戰的影響。玄奘大學應用心理所碩士論文。
魯忠義、彭建國、李強(2003)。中文故事閱讀中預期與前後向推理的關係。心理學報,35,183-189。
劉政宏(2008)。你贊成或反對立場對立者的論點?「立場對立情境論點贊否模式(CSAAM)」之驗證。國科會專題研究計畫(編號:NSC 97-2410-H-364-007)。
Abele, A. (1985). Thinking about thinking: Causal, evaluative and finalistic cognitions about social situations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 315-332.
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1962). Performance expectancy as a determinant of actual performance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 178-182.
Balcetis, E. (2009). Claiming a moral minority, saccades help create a biased majority: Tracking eye movements to base rates in social predictions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 970-973.
Banaji, M. R., & Prentice, D. A. (1994). The self in social contexts. Annual review of psychology, 45, 297-332.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psvchological Review, 84, 191-215.
Bartholow, B. D., Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., & Bettencourt, B. A. (2001). A psychophysiological examination of cognitive processing of and affective responses to social expectancy violations. Psychological Science, 12, 197-204.
Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 680-740). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., & Finkenauer, C. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323-370.
Baumeister, R. F., & Cairns, K. J. (1992). Repression and self-presentation: When audiences interfere with self-deceptive strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 851-862.
Beauregard, K. S., & Dunning, D. (1998). Turning up the contrast: Self-enhancement motives prompt egocentric contrast effects in social judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 606-621.
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Quintanar, L. R. (1982). Individual difference in relative hemispheric alpha abundance and cognitive responses to persuasive communications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 623-636.
Campbell, W. K., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Review of General Psychology, 3, 23-43.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Apfel, N., & Brzustoski, P. (2009). Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation Intervening to Close the Minority Achievement Gap. Science, 324, 400-403.
Cole, S., & Balcetis, E. (2011). Of Visions and Desires: Biased Perceptions of the Environment can Serve Self-Protective Functions. In M. D. Alicke, & C. Sedikides (Eds.), Handbook of Self-Enhancement and Self-Protection (pp. 155-173). New York: The Guilford Press.
Critcher, C. R., Dunning, D., & Armor, D. A. (2010). When self-affirmations reduce defensiveness: Timing is key. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 947-959.
Ditto, P. H., & Lopez, D. F. (1992). Motivated skepticism: The use of differential decision criteria for preferred and nonpreferred conclusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 568-584.
Dunning, D., Meyerowitz, J. A., & Holzberg, A. D. (1989). Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability. Journal of personality and social psychology, 57, 1082-1090.
Fein, S., & Spencer, S. J. (1997). Prejudice as self-image maintenance: Affirming the self through derogating others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 31-44.
Fiske, S. T. (1980). Attention and weight in person perception: The impact of negative and extreme behavior. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 22, 889-906.
Graziano, W. G., Brothen, T., & Berscheid, E. (1980). Attention, attraction, and individual differences in reaction to criticism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 193-202.
Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, S. J. (1996). Perceiving persons and groups. Psychological Review, 103, 336–355.
Harris, P. R., & Napper, L. (2005). Self-affirmation and the biased processing of threatening health-risk information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1250–1263.
Hastie, R. (1984). Causes and effects of causal attributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 44-56.
Hattie, J. (1999). Influences on student learning. Inaugural lecture given on August, 2, 1999.Available at URL: http://xn--www-rp0a.teacherstoolbox.co.uk/downloads/ managers/Influencesonstudent.pdf
Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Review of educational research, 77, 81-112.
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Jacobson, N. S. (1985). Causal attributions of marital couples: When do they search for causes? What do they conclude when they do? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1398-1412.
Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 887-900.
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of memory and language, 30, 513-541.
Kanouse, D. E., & Hanson, L. R. (1972). Negativity in evaluations. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 47-62). Morristown, NJ: General Learning.
Koole, S. L., Smeets, K., van Knippenberg, A., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 111-125.
Koole, S. L. & van Knippenberg, A. (2007). Controlling your mind without ironic consequences: Self-affirmation eliminates rebound effects after thought suppression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 671-677.
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological bulletin, 108, 480-498.
Kunda, Z. (1999). Social Cognition: Making sense of people. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kwang, T., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2010). Do people embrace praise even when they feel unworthy? A review of critical tests of self-enhancement versus self-verification. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 263-280.
Ohira, H., Winton, W. M., & Oyama, M. (1998). Effects of stimulus valence on recognition memory and endogenous eyeblinks: Further evidence for positive-negative asymmetry. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 986-993.
Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 33-60). New York: Wiley.
Pezzo, M. V. (2003). Surprise, defence, or making sense: What removes hindsight bias? Memory, 11, 421-441.
Pezzo, M. V., & Pezzo, S. P. (2007). Making Sense of Failure: A Motivated Model of Hindsight Bias. Social Cognition, 25, 147-164.
Robinson-Riegler, G. L., & Winton, W. M. (1996). The role of conscious recollection in recognition of affective material: Evidence for positive-negative asymmetry. Journal of General Psychology, 123, 93-104.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Sanna, L. J., & Turley, K. J. (1996). Antecedents to spontaneous counterfactural thinking: Effects of expectancy violation and outcome valence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 906-919.
Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293-317). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: A review and model for future research . Psychological Bulletin, 99, 143-165.
Scholl, R. W. (1981). Differentiating organizational commitment from expectancy as a motivating force. Academy of Management Review, 6, 589-599.
Schwinghammer S. A., Stapel, D. A., & Blanton, H. (2006). Different Selves Have Different Effects: Self-Activation and Defensive Social Comparison. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 27-39.
Sedikides, C. (1993). Assessment, enhancement, and verification determinants of the self-evaluation process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 317-338.
Sedikides, C., & Green, J. D. (2000). On the self-protective nature of inconsistency-negativity management: Using the person memory paradigm to examine self-referent memory. Journal of personality and social psychology, 79, 906-922.
Sedikides, C., Green, J. D., & Pinter, B. (2004). Self-protective memory. In D. R. Beike, J. M. Lampinen, & D. A. Behrend (Eds.), The self and memory (pp. 161-179). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (2000). On the evolutionary functions of the symbolic self: The emergence of self-evaluation motives. In A. Tesser, R. B. Felson, & J. M. Suls (Eds.), Psyhological perspectives on self and identity (pp. 91-117). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine own self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thine own self be better. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, 29, 209-269.
Sherman, D. K. & Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of self-defense: Self-Affirmation Theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 183-242.
Sherman, D. K., Cohen, G. L., Nelson, L. D., Nussbaum, A. D., Bunyan, D. P., & Garcia, J. (2009). Affirmed yet unaware: Exploring the role of awareness in self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 745-764.
Sherman, D. K., & Hartson, K. A. (2011). Reconciling Self-Protection With Self-Improvement: Self-Affirmation Theory. In M. D. Alicke, & C. Sedikides (Eds.), Handbook of Self-Enhancement and Self-Protection (pp. 128-151). New York: The Guilford Press.
Sherman, D. K., Nelson, L. D., & Steele, C. M. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten the self? Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1046–1058.
Skowronski, J. J., Betz, A. L., Thompson, C. P., & Shannon, L. (1991). Social memory in everyday life: Recall of self-events and other-events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 831-843.
Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1989). Person memory and judgment. Psychological Review, 96, 58-83.
Stapel, D. A., & van der Linde, L. A. J. G. (2011). What Drives Self-Affirmation Effects? On the Importance of Differentiating Value Affirmation and Attribute Affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 34-45.
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self‐affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261–302). New York: Academic Press.
Steele, C. M., & Liu, T. J. (1983). Dissonance processes as self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 5-19.
Swann, W. B., Jr., & Read, S. J. (1981). Acquiring self-knowledge-The search for feedback that fits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 1119-1128.
Swann, W. B., Jr., Stein-Seroussi, A., & Giesler, R. B. (1992). Why people self-verify. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 392-401.
Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67-85.
Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 181–227). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Vohs, K. D., Park, J. K., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2013). Self-affirmation can enable goal disengagement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 14-27.
Weiner, B. (1985). “Spontaneous” causal thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 74-84.
Wong, P. T., & Weiner, B. (1981). When people ask ‘Why’ questions and the heuristics of attributional search. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 650-663.