簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 蔡銘仁
Tsai, Min-jen
論文名稱: 以表現為基礎的回饋安排在動作表現與學習效應:自我控制與寬帶回饋的延伸
Effects of Performance-based Feedback Scheduling on Motor Performance and Learning: Extension of Self-controlled and Bandwidth Feedback
指導教授: 卓俊伶
Jwo, Jun-Ling
學位類別: 博士
Doctor
系所名稱: 體育學系
Department of Physical Education
論文出版年: 2016
畢業學年度: 104
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 155
中文關鍵詞: 自我調節動作表現訊息處理結果獲知錯誤偵查
英文關鍵詞: self-regulation, motor performance,, information-processing, knowledge of results, error detection
DOI URL: https://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202204436
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:192下載:15
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 本研究旨在探討依據表現提供回饋於動作表現與學習的效應。實驗一延續過去自我控制回饋研究發現,在非自我控制條件下對於誤差方向判斷錯誤的試作提供結果獲知,檢驗對於動作學習的效應;實驗二檢驗自我控制回饋的練習條件下檢驗安排寬帶回饋在動作表現與學習的效應。兩個實驗各招募24名成年人為實驗參與者 (實驗一16名男性,8名女性,平均年齡24.6 ± 2.2歲;實驗二18名男性,6名女性,平均年齡23.7 ± 1.9歲),以滑塊推滑 (puck-shuffling) 的力量控制動作為實驗工作。結果發現在非自我控制回饋的情境中於針對誤差方向判斷錯誤的試作提供回饋,並沒有產生動作表現與學習作用,而在自我控制條件下結合寬帶回饋則對於動作學習穩定度則有正面效益。誤差判斷錯誤的試作的實際動作表現較判斷正確的試作準確,且主觀估計的動作表現亦低於誤差判斷正確的試作準確;在自我控制的情境中,學習者要求回饋的試作動作表現較不要求回饋的試作準確,對於練習過程中的限制學習者可能採取自我調節學習策略。透過比對主觀與客觀動作表現誤差結果發現,學習者對於自我表現具有高估的現象。

    This study was designed to examine effects of motor performance and learning, based on performance-based feedback schedules. In experiment 1, by extending the findings of self-controlled feedback, feedback was provided while learners made wrong judgment of error direction in a non-self-controlled condition, which examined the effect of motor learning. Experiment 2 examined the effects of motor performance and learning by adding bandwidth feedback in self-controlled context. Both of two experiments of the present study recruited twenty-four adults as participants respectively (Experiment 1: 16 males, 8 females, mean age: 24.6 ± 2.2 yrs.; Experiment 2: 18 males, 6 females, mean age: 23.7 ± 1.9 yrs.). The two experiments adopted a force control puck-shuffling movement as experimental task. The results revealed that providing feedback for trials that learners made wrong judgment of error direction did not result in the effects of motor performance and learning in a non-self-controlled feedback condition. Combining bandwidth feedback in self-controlled condition resulted in motor learning benefits in stability. The actual motor performance and subjectively estimated motor performance of trials that learners could not correctly judge the direction of error was more accurate than the trials that learners made judgment correctly; The motor performance of trials that learners requested feedback was more accurate than the ones that they did not request in self-controlled condition. Learners might adopted self-regulated learning strategies to cope the constraints they suffered in learning process. By comparing the subjective error and objective error of motor performance, the results indicated that learners tended to over-estimated their performance.

    目 次 頁次 口試委員與系主任論文通過簽名表…………………………………………………i 論文授權書……………………………………………………………………………ii 中文摘要……………………………………………………………………...………iii 英文摘要……………………………………………………………………………...iv 謝誌………………………………………………………………………………...….v 目次…………………………………………………………………………………...vi 圖次………………………………………………………………………………….viii 表次…………………………………………………………………………………...ix 第壹章 緒論……………………………………………………………………..1 第一節 問題背景……………………………………………………………………..2 第二節 研究目的……………………………………………………………………..7 第三節 研究的基本假定……………………………………………………………..8 第四節 研究範圍與限制……………………………………………………………..9 第五節 名詞操作性定義……………………………………………………………10 第六節 研究的重要性………………………………………………………………14 第貳章 文獻探討…………………………………………………………….15 第一節 動作學習的理論……………………………………………………………15 第二節 回饋的理論基礎……………………………………………………………22 第三節 自我控制回饋的相關研究………………………………………………....28 第四節 寬帶回饋的相關研究………………………………………………………45 第五節 文獻探討小結…………………………………………………..…………50 第參章 實驗一 依據誤差方向判斷結果提供回饋的動作表現與學習效應….52 第一節 研究問題與假說…………………..………………………………………..52 第二節 方法…………………………………………………………………………53 第三節 結果…………………………………………………………………………62 第四節 討論…………………………………………………………………………72 第肆章 實驗二 寬帶回饋在自我控制回饋情境的動作學習附加效益….….…..86 第一節 研究問題與假說……………………………………………………………87 第二節 方法…………………………………………………………………………88 第三節 結果…………………………………………………………………………94 第四節 討論………………………………………………………………………..102 第伍章 綜合討論……………………………………………………………113 第陸章 結論與建議………………………………………………………...117 第一節 結論………………………………………………………………………..117 第二節 建議………………………………………………………………….…….118 參考文獻………………………………………………………………………...120 中文部分……………………………………………………………………………120 西文部分……………………………………………………………………………121 附錄…………………………………………………..………………………...…135 附錄一 研究參與者知情同意書 (實驗一)…………………………………….…136 附錄二 研究參與者知情同意書 (實驗二)…………………………………….…139 附錄三 實驗一各變項描述統計與摘要表…………………………………….…142 附錄四 實驗二各變項描述統計與摘要表……………………………………….150 圖 次 圖1 實驗設備示意圖………………………………………………………………56 圖2 實驗一流程圖……………………………………………………….…………60 圖3 實驗二流程圖………………………………………………………………….92 圖4 獲得期各區間回饋提供頻率………………………………………………….63 圖5 獲得期及保留、遷移測驗絕對誤差值曲線圖……………………………….65 圖6 獲得期及保留、遷移測驗變異誤差值曲線圖……………………………….66 圖7 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組獲得期各區間有無提供回饋 試作動作表現絕對誤差值…..……………………………………………….68 圖8 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組獲得期各區間 有無提供回饋試作估計絕對誤差值…...………………………………….…69 圖9 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組獲得期各區間 提供回饋試作動作表現與估計絕對誤差值...…………………………….…70 圖10 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組獲得期各區間 無提供回饋試作動作表現與估計絕對誤差值…...………………………….71 圖11 自我控制+寬帶組獲得期各區間質性回饋頻率…………………………….98 圖12 獲得期各區間回饋要求頻率…………………………………………………96 圖13 獲得期及保留、遷移測驗絕對誤差值曲線圖………………………………98 圖14 獲得期及保留、遷移測驗變異誤差值曲線圖………………………………..99 圖15 自我控制+寬帶組獲得期各區間回饋要求與否試作絕對誤差值…………101 圖16 自我控制組獲得期各區間回饋要求與否試作絕對誤差值………………..102 表 次 實驗一各變項描述統計與摘要表 表1 實驗一各區間回饋提供比率平均數與標準差………………………………142 表2 獲得期合併區間頻率相依樣本變異數分析摘要表…………………………142 表3 獲得期合併區間頻率主要效果的事後比較摘要表…………………………142 表4 獲得期及保留、遷移測驗絕對誤差值平均數與標準差……………………143 表5 獲得期絕對誤差值混合設計二因子變異數分析摘要表……………………143 表6 獲得期絕對誤差值區間因子事後比較摘要表………………………………143 表7 保留測驗絕對誤差值獨立樣本t考驗摘要表………………………………..144 表8 遷移測驗絕對誤差值獨立樣本t考驗摘要表………………………………..144 表9 獲得期及保留、遷移測驗變異誤差值平均數與標準差……………………144 表10 獲得期變異誤差值混合設計二因子變異數分析摘要表………………..…144 表11 獲得期變異誤差值區間因子事後比較摘要表……………………………..145 表12 保留測驗變異誤差值獨立樣本t考驗摘要表……………………….……...145 表13 遷移測驗變異誤差值獨立樣本t考驗摘要表………………………………145 表14 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組要求回饋與否 動作表現絕對誤差值平均數與標準差……………………………………..146 表15 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組回饋要求與否動作表現 絕對誤差值相依樣本二因子變異數分析摘要表…………………..………146 表16 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組回饋要求與否動作表現 絕對誤差值依樣本二因子變異數分析區間因子事後比較摘要表……..…146 表17 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組估計絕對誤差值平均數與標準差…………..…147 表18 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組回饋要求與否估計絕對誤差值 相依樣本二因子變異數分析摘要表…………………...……………….…..147 表19 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組提供回饋試作實際表現與估計絕對誤差值 平均數與標準差………………..……………………………………………147 表20 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組提供回饋試作實際表現與估計絕對誤差值 相依樣本二因子變異數分析摘要表………………………..………………147 表21 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組提供回饋試作實際表現與估計絕對誤差值 相依樣本二因子變異數區間因子事後比較摘要表……………………..…148 表22 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組無提供回饋試作實際表現與估計絕對誤差值 平均數與標準差……………………………………………………………..148 表23 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組無提供回饋試作實際表現與估計絕對誤差值 相依樣本變異數分析摘要表……………………………………………..…148 表24 誤差方向判斷錯誤回饋組無提供回饋試作實際表現與估計絕對誤差值 相依樣本二因子變異數區間因子事後比較摘要表……………………..…149 實驗二各變項描述統計與摘要表 表25 自我控制+寬帶組與自我控制回饋組各區間回饋要求比率………………150 表26 自我控制+寬帶組與自我控制回饋組各區間回饋要求比率 混合設計二因子變異數分析摘要表…………………………………..……150 表27 自我控制+寬帶組提供質性回饋佔要求回饋與試作比率…………………150 表28 自我控制+寬帶組提供質性回饋佔要求佔回饋比率 單因子變異數分析摘要表…………………………..………………………151 表29 自我控制+寬帶組提供質性回饋佔試作比率 單因子變異數分析摘要表………………………………………………......151 表30 獲得期及保留、遷移測驗絕對誤差值平均數與標準差……………………151 表31 獲得期絕對誤差值混合設計二因子變異數分析摘要表………………..…152 表32 獲得期絕對誤差值區間因子事後比較摘要表……………………………..152 表33 保留測驗絕對誤差值獨立樣本t考驗摘要表………………………………152 表34 遷移測驗絕對誤差值獨立樣本t考驗摘要表………………………………152 表35 獲得期及保留、遷移測驗變異誤差值平均數與標準差……………………153 表36 獲得期變異誤差值混合設計二因子變異數分析摘要表…………………..153 表37 獲得期變異誤差值區間因子事後比較摘要表…………………………..…153 表38 保留測驗變異誤差值獨立樣本t考驗摘要表………………………………154 表39 遷移測驗變異誤差值獨立樣本t考驗摘要表………………………………154 表40 自我控制+寬帶組動作表現絕對誤差值平均數與標準差…………………154 表41 自我控制+寬帶組回饋要求與否試作動作表現 相依樣本二因子變異數分析摘要表……………………………………..…154 表42 自我控制+寬帶組回饋要求與否試作動作表現 相依樣本二因子變異數分析區間因子事後比較摘要表………………..…155 表43 自我控制組動作表現絕對誤差值平均數與標準差……………………..…155 表44 自我控制組回饋要求與否試作動作表現 相依樣本二因子變異數分析摘要表………………………………..………155 表45 自我控制組回饋要求與否試作動作表現 相依樣本二因子變異數分析區間因子事後比較摘要表……..……………155

    中文部分

    方正銘、卓俊伶 (2008)。動作表現與學習的帶寬回饋及年齡效應。臺灣運動心
    理學報,13,21-38。

    李淑華、林靜兒 (2013)。自我控制回饋促進動作技能學習之因素。中華體育季
    刊,27,335-342。

    吳詩薇、張至滿、卓俊伶、蔡銘仁 (2016)。摘要結果獲知的時近訊息有利於自
    我控制學習。大專體育學刊,18,14-26。

    林尚武、卓俊伶、楊梓楣、陳重佑、葉俊良 (2009)。自發性錯誤估計促進自我
    控制回饋的動作學習效益。體育學報,42,15-28.

    胡名霞 (2009)。動作控制與動作學習。臺北市:金名。

    張智惠、卓俊伶 (1998)。不同情境干擾對自我配速動作空間準確性及錯誤偵查
    能力學習的影響。體育學報,7,209-218。

    張櫻玉、卓俊伶 (2003)。錯誤估計與結果獲知對空間移動性動作表現與學習的
    影響。臺灣運動心理學報,2,77-92。

    陳玉芬、卓俊伶 (1998)。特定範圍結果獲知對高爾夫推桿動作之空間準確性與
    錯誤偵查的影響。體育學報,7,249-258。

    葉俊良、卓俊伶、林靜兒、陳重佑 (2007)。自我控制回饋對空間性動作表現、
    學習及錯誤估計的效應。大專體育學刊,9,23-35。

    蔡銘仁、卓俊伶 (準備中a)。Better performance results in feedback requests.

    蔡銘仁、卓俊伶 (準備中b)。Feedback provided within the learners-regulated
    bandwidth endorses motor learning effect of self-controlled feedback.

    顧毓群 (1989)。閉鎖環動作學習理論之評析。中華體育季刊,2,25-31。

    西文部分

    Adams, J. A. (1971). A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Journal of Motor
    Behavior, 3, 111-150.

    Adams, J. A., & Goetz, E. T. (1973). Feedback and practice as variables in error
    detection and correction. Journal of Motor Behavior, 5, 217-224.

    Ahmadi, P., Sabiz, A. H., Heirani, A., & Hasanvand, B. (2011). The effect of feedback
    after good, poor, good-poor trials, and self-control conditions in an
    acquisition and learning of force production task. Physical Education and
    Sport, 9, 35-43.

    Aiken, C. A., Fairbrother, J. T., & Post, P. G. (2012). The effects of self-controlled
    video feedback on the learning of the basketball set shot. Frontiers in
    Psychology, 3, 1-8.

    Alikhani, H., Gharaat, M. A., Naghdi, G. N., Abarghni, L. E., & Alikhani, M. (2013).
    Effect of error detection and time of decision making for request feedback in
    self-controlled conditions acquisition and retention of a complex task. Annals
    of Biological Research, 4, 22-29.

    Anderson, D. I., Magill, R. A., & Sekiya, H. (2001). Motor learning as a function of
    KR schedule and charateristics of task-intrinsic feedback. Journal of Motor
    Behavior, 33, 59-66.

    Andrieux, M., Danna, J., Thon, B. (2012). Self-control of task difficulty during
    training enhances motor learning of a complex coincidence-anticipation task.
    Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 83, 27-35.

    Annett, J. (1969). Feedback and human behavior. Middlesex, England: Penguin.

    Ávila, L. T. G., Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2012). Positive
    social-comparative feedback enhances motor learning in children. Psychology
    of Sport and Exercise, 13, 849-853.

    Badami, R., VaezMousavi, M., Wulf, G., & Namazizadeh, M. (2011). Feedback after
    good versus poor trials affects intrinsic motivation. Research Quarterly for
    Exercise and Sport, 82, 360-364.

    Badami, R., VaezMousavi, M., Wulf, G., & Namazizadeh, M. (2012). Feedback about
    more accurate versus less accurate trials: Differential effects on
    self-confidence and activation. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 83,
    196-203.

    Badets, A., & Blandin, Y. (2004). The role of knowledge of results frequency in
    learning through observation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 62-70.

    Badets, A., & Blandin, Y. (2005). Observational learning: Effects of bandwidth
    knowledge of results. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37, 211-216.

    Badets, A., Blandin, Y., Wright, D. L., & Shea, C. H. (2006). Error detection processes
    during observational learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 77,
    177-184.

    Bagherli, J., Mohsenzadeh, M., Sabzi, A. H., & Fotrousi, F. (2013). Bandwidth
    feedback: Does it effective when provided near or far from target on learning
    of putting skill? Middle-East Journal of Scientific Research, 13, 1-4.

    Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and actions: A social cognitive
    theory. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

    Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology.
    Cambridge: Cambridge University.

    Bilodeau, E. A., & Bilodeau, I. M. (1985). Variable frequency knowledge of results
    and the learning of simple skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55,
    379-383.

    Braten, I. (1991). Vygotsky as precursor to metacogntive theory: I. The concept of
    metacogntion and its roots. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research,
    35, 179-192.

    Broujeni, E. P., Shojaei, M., & Daneshfar, A. (2014). Effects of feedback after
    successful trials, normative feedback, and self-controlled feedback on learning
    of badminton service. International Journal of Sport Studies, 4, 823-829.

    Bruechert, L., Lai, Q., & Shea, C. H. (2003). Reduced knowledge of results frequency
    enhances error detection. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74,
    467-472.

    Butler, M. S., Reeve, T. G., & Fischman, M. G. (1996). Effects of the instructional set
    in the bandwidth feedback paradigm on motor skill acquisition. Research
    Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67, 355-359.

    Carpenter, S. (2001). A blind spot in motor learning. Monitor on Psychology, 32, 1-5.

    Carter, M. J., & Patterson, J. T. (2012). Self-controlled knowledge of results:
    Age-related differences in motor learning, strategies, and error detection.
    Human Movement Science, 31, 1459-1472.

    Carter, M. J., Smith, V., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2016). Judgments of learning are
    significantly higher following feedback on relatively good versus relatively
    poor trials despite no actual learning differences. Human Movement Science,
    45, 63-70.

    Cauraugh, J. H., Chen, D., & Radlo, S. J. (1993). Effects of traditional and reversed
    bandwidth knowledge of results on motor learning. Research Quarterly for
    Exercise and Sport, 64, 413-417.

    Chen, D. D., Hendrick, J. L., & Lidor, R. (2002). Enhancing self-controlled learning
    environment: The use of self-regulated feedback information. Journal of
    Human Movement Studies, 43, 69-86.

    Chen, D., & Singer, R. N. (1992). Self-regulation and cognitive strategies in sport
    participation. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 277-300.

    Chiviacowsky, S. (2014). Self-controlled practice: Autonomy protects perceptions of
    competence and enhances motor learning. Psychology of Sport and Exercise,
    15, 505-510.

    Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2002). Self-controlled feedback: Does it enhance
    learning because performers get feedback when they need it? Research
    Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 73, 408-415.

    Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2005). Self-controlled feedback is effective if it is
    based on the learners’ performance. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
    Sport, 76, 42-48.

    Chiviacowsky, S., & Wulf, G. (2007). Feedback after good trials enhances learning.
    Research Quarterly Exercise and Sport, 78, 40-47.

    Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2012). Self-controlled learning: The
    importance of protecting perceptions of competence. Frontiers in Psychology,
    3, 1-8.

    Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., Mederiros, F. L., Kaefer, A., & Tani, G. (2008). Learning
    benefits of self-controlled knowledge of results in 10-year-old children.
    Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79, 405-410.

    Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., Mederios, F., Kaefer, A., & Wally, R. (2008).
    Self-controlled feedback in children: Higher feeddback frequencies enhance learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79, 122-127.

    Chiviacowsky, S., Wulf, G., Wally, R., & Borges, T. (2009). Knowledge of results
    after good trials enhances learning in older adults. Research Quarterly for
    Exercise and Sport, 80, 663-668.

    Coca-Ugrinowitsch, A. A., & Ugrinowitsch, H. (2014). Bandwidth knowledge of
    results on the learning of the saloon dart throwing task. Perceptual and Motor
    Skills, 118, 462-474.

    Dail, T. K., & Christina, R. W. (2004). Distribution of practice and metacognition in
    learning and long-term retention of a discrete motor task. Research Quarterly
    for Exercise and Sport, 75, 148-155.

    Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of external mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation.
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105-115.

    Deci, E. L. (1972). Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic reinforcement, and inequity. Journal
    of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 113-120.

    Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.

    Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. N. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in
    human behavior. NY: Plenum Press.

    Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. N. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human
    needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11,
    227-268.

    Fairbrother, J. T., Laughlin, D. D., & Nguyen, T. V. (2012). Self-controlled feedback
    facilitates motor learning in both high and low activity individuals. Frontiers
    in Psychology, 31, 1-7.

    Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of
    cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906-911.

    Fogarty, G., & Else, D. (2005). Performance calibration in sport: Implications for
    self-confidence and metacognition biases. International Journal of Sport and
    Exercise Psychology, 3, 41-57.

    Goodwin, J. E., Meeuwsen, H. J. (1995). Using bandwidth knowledge of results to
    alter relative frequencies during motor skill acquisition. Research Quarterly
    for Exercise and Sport, 66, 99-104.

    Green, S., & Sherwood, D. E. (2000). The benefits of random variable practice for
    accuracy and temporal error detection in a rapid aiming task. Research
    Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 398-402.

    Guadagnoli, M. A., & Kohl, R. M. (2001). Knowledge of results for motor learning:
    Relationship between error estimation and knowledge of results frequency.
    Journal of Motor Behavior, 33, 217-224.

    Guadagnoli, M. A., & Lee, T. D. (2004). Challenge point: A framework for conceptualizing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 212-224.
    Hadadi, N., & Kheirjoo, E. (2012). European Journal of Experimental Biology, 2,
    1193-1199.

    Hartman, J. M. (2007). Self-controlled use of a perceived physical assistance device during a balancing task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 104, 1005-1016.

    Hansen, S., Pfeiffer, J., & Patterson, J. D. (2011). Self-control of feedback during motor learning: Accounting for the absolute amount of feedback using a yoked group with self-control over feedback. Journal of Motor Behavior, 43, 113-119.

    Hogan, J. C., & Yanowitz, B. A. (1978). The role of verbal estimates of movement
    error in ballistic skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 10, 133-138.

    Hooyman, A., Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2014). Impacts of autonomy-supportive
    versus controlling instructional language on motor learning. Human Movement
    Science, 36, 190-198.

    Inzlicht, M., Legault, L., Teper, R. (2014). Exploring the mechanisms of self-control
    improvement. Current Directions in Psychology Science, 23, 302-307.

    Ishikura, I. (2008). Reduced relative frequency of knowledge of results without visual
    feedback in learning a golf-putting task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 106,
    225-233.

    Janelle, C. M., Barba, D. A., Frehlich, S. G., Tannant, L. K., & Cauraugh, J. H. (1997). Maximizing performance feedback effectiveness through videotape replay and a self-controlled learning environment. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 68, 269-279.

    Janelle, C. M., Kim, J., & Singer, R. N. (1995). Subject-controlled performance
    feedback and learning of a closed motor skill. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81,
    1627-634.

    Kaefer, A., Chiviacowsky, S., Meira, C. M., & Tani, G. (2014). Self-controlled
    practice enhances motor learning in introverts and extroverts. Research
    Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 85, 226-233.

    Keetch, K. M., & Lee, T. D. (2007). The effect of self-regulated and
    experimenter-imposed practice schedules on motor learning for tasks of
    varying difficulty. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 476-486.

    Lai, Q., & Shea, C. H. (1999). Bandwidth knowledge of results enhances generalized
    motor program learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 70,
    79-83.

    Lai, Q., & Shea, C. H. (1999). The role of reduced frequency of knowledge of results
    during constant practice. Research Quarterly for Exercise and sport, 70,
    33-40.

    Lashley, K. S. (1917). The accuracy of movement in the absence of excitation from
    the moving organ. American Journal of Physiology, 43, 169-194.

    Lee, T. D., & Carnahan, H. (1990). Bandwidth knowledge of results and motor
    learning: More than just a relative frequency effect. The Quarterly Journal of
    Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 42,
    777-789.

    Lee, T. D., & Maraj, B. K. V. (1994). Effects of bandwidth goals and bandwidth
    knowledge of results on motor learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
    Sport, 65, 244-249.

    Lee, T. D., Swinnen, S. P., & Serrien, D. J. (1994). Cognitive effort and motor
    learning. Quest, 46, 328-344.

    Lee, T. D., White, M. A., & Carnahan, H. (1990). The role of knowlwdge of results in
    motor learning: Exploring the guidance hypothesis. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22, 191-208.
    Lee, T. D., & Wishart, L. R. (2005). Motor learning conundrums and possible
    solutions. Quest, 57, 67-78.

    Lewthwaite, R., Chiviacowsky, S., Drews, R., & Wulf, G. (2015). Choose to move:
    The motivational impact of autonomy support on motor learning.
    Psychoonomic Bulletin and Review, 22, 1383-1388.

    Lewthwaite, R., & Wulf, G. (2010). Social-comparative feedback affects motor skill
    learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 738-749.

    Lui, J., & Wrisberg, C. A. (1997). The effect of knowledge of results delay and
    subjective estimation of movement form on the acquisition and retention of a
    motor skill. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 68, 145-151.

    MaRae, M., Patterson, J. D., & Hansen, S. (2015). Examining the preferred self-controlled KR schedules on learners and peers during motor skill learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 47, 527-534.

    Magill, R. A., & Anderson, D. I. (2012). The roles and uses of augmented feedback in motor skill acquisition. In N. J. Hodges & A. M. Williams (Eds). Skill acquisition- Research, theory and practice (2nd ed) (pp.3-21.). New York: Routledge.

    Magill, R. A. (1994). The influence of augmented feedback on skill learning depends on characteristics of the skill and the learner. Quest, 46, 314-327.

    Magill, R. A. (2011). Motor learning and control: Concepts and applications. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Magill, R. A., Chamberlin, C. J., & Hall, K. G. (1991). Verbal knowledge of result as
    redundant information for learning an anticipation timing task. Human Movement Science, 10, 485-507.

    Magill, R. A., & Wood, C. (1986). Knowledge of results precision as a learning in
    motor skill acquisition. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 57,
    170-173.

    Marteniuk, R. G. (1976). Information processing in motor skills. New York: Holt,
    Rinehart, & Winston.

    Medin, D. L., Ross, B. N., & Markman, A. B. (2004). Cognitive psychology (4th ed.).
    New Jersey: Wiley and Sons.

    Memmert, D. (2006). Self-controlled practice of decision-making skills. Perceptual
    and Motor Skills, 103, 879-882.
    Park, J. H., Shea, C. H., & Wright, D. L. (2000). Reduced-frequency concurrent and
    terminal feedback: A test a guidance hypothesis. Journal of Motor Behavior,
    32, 287-296.

    Patterson, J. T., & Azizieh, J. (2012). Knowing the good from bad: Does being aware
    of KR content matter? Human Movement Science, 31, 1449-1458.

    Patterson, J. T., & Carter, M. (2010). Learner regulated knowledge of results during
    the acquisition of multiple timing goals. Human Movement Science, 29,
    214-227.

    Patterson, J. T., Carter, M., & Sanli, E. (2011). Decreasing the proportion of
    self-control trials during the acquisition period does not compromise the
    learning advantages in a self-controlled context. Research Quarterly for
    Exercise and Sport, 82, 624-633.

    Patterson, J. T., & Lee, T. D. (2010). Self-regulated frequency of augmented
    information in skill learning. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,
    64, 33-40.

    Pintrich, P. B. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In M.
    Boekaert, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regualtion (pp. 451-502). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of
    intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American
    Psychologist, 55, 68-78.

    Sadowski, J., Mastaler, A., Niznikowski, T. (2013). Benefits of bandwidth feedback in
    learning a complex gymnastic skill. Journal of Human Kinetics, 37, 183-193.

    Salmoni, A. W., Schmidt, R. A., & Walter, C. B. (1984). Knowledge of results and
    motor learning: A review and critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 95,
    355-38.

    Sanli, E. A., & Patterson, J. T. (2013). Learning effects of self-controlled practice
    scheduling for children and adults: Are the advantages different? Perceptual
    and Motor Skills, 116, 741-749.

    Sanli, E. A., Patterson, J. T., Bray, S. R., & Lee, T. D. (2012). Understanding
    self-controlled motor learning protocols through the self-determination theory.
    Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1-17.

    Sadowski, J., Mastalerz, A., & Niznikowski, T. (2013). Benefits of bandwidth
    feedback in learning a complex gymnastic skill. Journal of Human Kinetics,
    37, 183-193.

    Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning.
    Psychological Review, 82, 225-260.

    Schmidt, R. A. (1991). Frequent augmented feedback can degrade learning: Evidence
    and interpretations. In J. Requin & G. E. Stelmach (Eds.). Tutorials in motor
    neuroscience (pp. 59-75). Dordrecht, Netherland: Kluwer.

    Schmidt, R. A., & Bjork, R. A., (1992). New conceptualizations of practice: Common
    principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training.
    Psychological Science, 3, 207-217.

    Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (2011). Motor control and learning: A behavioral
    emphasis (5th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Schmidt, R. A., & White, J. L. (1972). Evidence for an error detection mechanism in
    motor skills: A test of Adams’ closed-loop theory. Journal of Motor Behavior,
    4, 143-153.

    Schmidt, R. A., & Wrisberg, C. A. (1973). Further tests of adams’ closed-loop theory:
    Response-produced feedback and the error detection mechanism. Journal of
    Motor Behavior, 5, 155-164.

    Schmidt, R. A., Young, D. E., Shapiro, D. C., & Swinnen, S. (1989). Summary
    knowledge of results for skill acquisition: Support for the guidance hypothesis.
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15,
    352-359.

    Schunk, D. H. (2005). Self-regulated learning: The educational legacy of Paul R. Pintrich. Eduactional Psychology, 40, 85-94.

    Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., & Meece, J. L. (2008). Motivation in education- Theory, research, and applications. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

    Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2008). Motivation and self-regulated learning-
    Theory, research, and applications. New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Shea, J. B., & Morgan, R. L. (1979). Contextual interference effects on the acquisition
    retention, and transfer of a motor skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
    Human Learning and Memory, 5, 179-187.

    Simon, D. A., & Bjork, R. A. (2001). Metacognition in motor learning. Journal of
    Experimental Psychology- Learning Memory and Cognition, 27, 907-912.

    Simon, D. A., & Bjork, R. A. (2002). Models of performance in learning
    multisegment movement tasks: Consequences for acquisition, retention, and
    judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8,
    222-232.

    Sherwood, D. E. (1988). Effect of bandwidth knowledge of results on movement
    consistency. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66, 535-542.

    Sherwood, D. E. (2010). Detecting and correcting errors in rapid aiming movements:
    Effects of movement time, distance, and velocity. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 81, 300-309.

    Sherwood, D. E., & Lee, D. T. (2003). Schema theory: Critical review and
    implications for the role of cognition in a new theory of motor learning.
    Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74, 376-383.

    Smith, P. J. K., Taylor, S. J., & Withers, K. (1997). Applying bandwidth feedback
    scheduling to a golf shot. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 68,
    215-221.

    Swinnen, S. P., Schmidt, R. A., Nicholson, D. E., & Shapiro, D. C. (1990).
    Information feedback for skill acquisition: Instaneous knowledge of results
    degrades learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
    and Cognition, 16, 706-716.
    Thomas, J. R., Nelson, J. K., & Silverman, S. J. (2005). Research methods in physical
    activity ( 6th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Tompson, C. E., & Wankel, L. M. (1980). The effects of perceived activity choice
    upon frequency of exercise behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
    10, 436-443.

    Tsai, M. J., & Jwo, H. (2015). Controlling the absloute frequency of feedback in a
    self-controlled situation enhances motor learning. Perceptual and Motor
    Skills, 121, 746-758.

    Weeks, D. L., & Kordus, R. N. (1998). Relative frequency of knowledge of
    performance and motor skill learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
    Sport, 69, 224-230.

    Weeks, D. L., & Sherwood, D. E. (1994). A comparison of knowledge of results
    scheduling methods for promoting motor skill acquisition and retention.
    Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 65, 136-142.

    Winstein, C. J. (1991). Knowledge of results and motor learning- Implications for
    physical therapy. Physical Therapy, 71, 140-149.

    Winstein, C. J, & Schmidt, R. A. (1990). Reduced relative frequency of knowledge of
    results enhances motor learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
    Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 677-691.

    Wright, D. L., Smith-Munyon, V. L., & Sidaway, B. (1997). How close is too close for
    precise knowledge of results? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 68,
    172-176.

    Wright, D. L., Snowden, S., & Willoughby, D. (1990). Summary KR: How much
    information is used from the summary? Journal of Human Movement Studies,
    19, 119-128.

    Wulf, G., & Adams, N. (2014). Small choices can enhance balance learning. Human
    Movement Science, 38, 235-240.

    Wulf, G., Chiviacowsky, S., & Cardozo, P. L. (2014). Additive benefits of autonomy
    support and enhanced expectancies for motor learning. Human Movement
    Science, 37, 12-20.

    Wulf, G., Chiviacowsky, S., & Lewthwaite, R. (2010). Normative feedback effects on
    learning a timing task. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 81,
    425-431.

    Wulf, G., Freitas, H. E., & Tandy, R. D. (2014). Choosing to exercise more: Small
    choices increase exercise engagement. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15,
    268-271.

    Wulf, G., & Lewthwaite, R. (2016). Optimizing performance through intrinsic
    motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/c13423-015-0999-9.

    Wulf, G., Raupach, M., & Pfeiffer, F. (2005). Self-controlled observational practice
    enhances learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 76, 107-111.

    Wulf, G., & Schmidt, R. A. (1989). The learning of generalized motor programs:
    Reducing the relative frequency of knowledge of results enhances memory.
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15,
    748-757.

    Wulf, G., Shea, C., & Lewthwaite, R. (2010). Motor skill learning and performance: A
    review of influential factors. Medical Education, 44, 75-84.

    Wulf, G., & Toole, T. (1999). Physical assistance devices in complex motor skill
    learning: Benefits of a self-controlled practice schedule. Research Quarterly
    for Exercise and Sport, 70, 265-272.

    Yan, J. H., Thomas, J. R., & Thomas, K. T. (1998). Children’s age moderates the
    effect of practice variability: A quantitative review. Research Quarterly for
    Exercise and Sport, 69, 210-215.

    Young, D. E., & Schmidt, R. A. (1992). Augmented feedback for enhanced skill
    acquisition. In J. Requin & G. E. Stelmach (Eds.). Tutorials in motor
    Behavior II (pp. 677-693). Dordrecht, Netherland: Kluwer.

    Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Becoming a self-regulated learner: An overview. Theory into Practice, 41, 64-70.

    下載圖示
    QR CODE