研究生: |
簡珮如 Pei-Ju Chien |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
歌詞的語義理解:事件相關腦電位研究 Semantic Processing in Lyrics Perception: An ERP Study |
指導教授: |
詹曉蕙
Chan, Shiao-Hui |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
英語學系 Department of English |
論文出版年: | 2013 |
畢業學年度: | 101 |
語文別: | 英文 |
論文頁數: | 101 |
中文關鍵詞: | 事件相關腦電位 、歌詞 、語義理解歷程 、歌曲熟悉度 、N400 |
英文關鍵詞: | Event-related Potentials (ERPs), Song lyrics, Semantic Processing, Familiarity, N400 |
論文種類: | 學術論文 |
相關次數: | 點閱:212 下載:21 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
本文旨在以事件相關腦電位的技術來探討歌詞的語義理解歷程及歌曲熟悉度對此理解歷程之影響。過去研究語言與音樂的相關文獻指出,一般人在聽歌時,會對歌曲中的歌詞進行語義理解的分析,因而認為此語義分析與口語語言(spoken language)的理解歷程相似。然而,由於實驗目的不同及實驗操弄可能產生的混淆變項,關於歌詞的語義理解仍需更多討論。本實驗以跨感官知覺的語義促發作業(cross-modal semantic priming)為設計,在受試者聽完一段熟悉或不熟悉的中文流行歌曲片段後,對受試者以視覺方式呈現與該歌曲片段的句尾促發詞(prime)之語義相關或不相關的目標詞(target),並請受試者對於該目標詞進行名詞具體度的判斷。實驗結果發現,在目標詞與促發詞語義不相關的情況下,引發較語義相關為大的N400,顯示受試者在聽歌時,確實對於歌曲中的歌詞進行語義理解的分析。此外,在目標詞配對之促發詞為熟悉歌詞的情況下,N400的分布為中間偏左腦;然而,在目標詞配對之促發詞為不熟悉歌詞的情況下,N400的分布為中間偏右腦。此N400在左右腦分布上的差異顯示,受試者可能採取不同的語義分析策略來理解熟悉與不熟悉的歌詞。由於受試者對於熟悉的歌詞有較高的字詞期待(word expectancy),因此當受試者聽到熟悉的歌曲時,傾向用以左腦為主的預測式策略來進行歌詞理解。相較之下,受試者對於不熟悉的歌詞則有較低的字詞期待,加上中文歌詞的聲調(tone)在歌曲中因受到音樂影響而變得較不易辨識,因此當受試者聽到不熟悉的歌曲時,傾向用以右腦為主的整合式策略來進行歌詞理解。
Song lyrics are serving as a prevailing and important media communicating meanings in our daily life aside from written or spoken language. However, whether song lyrics are processed at a semantic level in song perception still remains less explored. While earlier studies investigating the relationship between lyrics and tune processing have reported that lyrics processing is conducted in the same way as spoken language which involves semantic processing, the results might have been confounded by methodological issues and thus could simply be task-induced effects. Therefore, the goal of the present study is to further explore lyrics processing with the ERP technique, testing whether people process lyrics semantically in song perception and whether song familiarity has an impact on such processing. A semantic priming paradigm was employed in the experiment. The subjects were aurally displayed a Chinese pop song excerpt before seeing a semantically related or unrelated target presented visually on the computer screen. Their task was to decide whether the target word is concrete or abstract. The results showed that an N400 effect was elicited in the comparison of semantically related vs. unrelated targets, suggesting that the subjects were processing the meaning of the lyrics stimuli in song perception. Furthermore, the ERP results showed a different pattern of N400 in hemispheric distribution between the familiar and the unfamiliar lyrics conditions: the N400 effect for familiar lyrics processing was significant in the midline and the left hemisphere, while for unfamiliar lyrics processing, the effect was significant in the midline and the right hemisphere. This hemispheric discrepancy of N400 indicated that the subjects might have used two different strategies in lyrics processing based on their familiarity toward the songs, prediction and integration. For the familiar lyrics, the subjects adopted a predictive manner in lyrics processing by bearing a relatively high word expectancy toward the lyrics. While for the unfamiliar lyrics, the subjects might be using an integrative strategy in lyrics processing due to their relatively low word expectancy on the lyrics and, more importantly, the obscure tonal feature of the lyrics in a song, which might have made it more difficult to access the semantic meaning of words. In summary, by examining the subjects' on-line processing in lyrics perception, the present study suggested that people process lyrics semantically in song perception and that they employ different strategies in processing songs with different familiarity.
Adrian, E. D., & Matthews, B. H. C. (1934). The Berger Rhythm: potential changes from the occipital lobes in man. Brain, 57, 355-384. doi: citeulike-article-id:4033846
Aguirre, G. K. (2007). Continuous carry-over designs for fMRI. NeuroImage, 35(4), 1480-1494. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.005
Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., & Wood, C. C. (1985). Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60(4), 343-355. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(85)90008-2
Berger, H. (1929). Über das Elektrenkephalogramm des Menschen. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 87(1), 527-570. doi: 10.1007/bf01797193
Besson, M., Faïta, F., Peretz, I., Bonnel, A.-M., & Requin, J. (1998). Singing in the brain: independence of lyrics and tunes. Psychological Science, 9, 494-498.
Bonnel, A.-M., Faita, F., Peretz, I., & Besson, M. (2001). Divided attention between lyrics and tunes of operatic songs: Evidence for independent processing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 63(7), 1201-1213. doi: 10.3758/bf03194534
Botvinick, M., & Plaut, D. C. (2004). Doing Without Schema Hierarchies: A Recurrent Connectionist Approach to Normal and Impaired Routine Sequential Action. Psychological Review, 111(2), 395-429. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.94.2.19210.1037/0033-295x.94.2.1921987-20973-001
10.1037/0096-3445.120.3.235183649010.1037/0096-3445.120.3.2351992-00389-001
10.1037/0033-295x.99.1.45.1992-16573-00110.1037/0033-295x.99.1.45
10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.830
Chao, Y. R. (1968). A Grammar of Spoken Chinese: Zhong Guo Hua de Wen Fa: University of California Press.
Chao, Y. R. (1969). Cantonese Primer. New York: Greenwood Press.
Chen-Hafteck, L. (1999). Discussing Text-Melody Relationship in Children's Song-Learning and Singing: A Cantonese-Speaking Perspective. Psychology of Music, 27(1), 55-70. doi: 10.1177/0305735699271006
Coles, M. G. H., & Rugg, M. D. (1995). Event-related brain potentials: An introduction. In M. D. Rugg & M. G. H. Coles (Eds.), Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-Related Brain Potentials and Cognition (pp. 1-26). London: Oxford University Press.
Connolly, J. F., & Phillips, N. A. (1994). Event-Related Potential Components Reflect Phonological and Semantic Processing of the Terminal Word of Spoken Sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(3), 256-266. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.3.256
Crowder, R., Serafine, M., & Repp, B. (1990). Physical interaction and association by contiguity in memory for the words and melodies of songs. Memory & Cognition,18(5), 469-476. doi: 10.3758/bf03198480
Domalski, P., Smith, M. E., & Halgren, E. (1991). Cross-Modal Repetition Effects on the N4. Psychological Science, 2(3), 173-178. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00126.x
Duncan, C. C., Barry, R. J., Connolly, J. F., Fischer, C., Michie, P. T., Näätänen, R., . . . Petten, C. V. (2009). Event-related potentials in clinical research: guidelines for eliciting, recording, and quantifying mismatch negativity, P300, and N400. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120, 1883-1908.
Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 44(4), 491-505. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00531.x
Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). Right words and left words: electrophysiological evidence for hemispheric differences in meaning processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 8(3), 373-392. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00036-1
Federmeier, K. D., McLennan, D. B., de Ochoa, E., & Kutas, M. (2002). The impact of semantic memory organization and sentence context information on spoken language processing by younger and older adults: An ERP study. Psychophysiology, 39(2), 133-146. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3920133
Federmeier, K. D., Wlotko, E. W., De Ochoa-Dewald, E., & Kutas, M. (2007). Multiple effects of sentential constraint on word processing. Brain research, 1146, 75-84.
Fendrick, P. (1937). The influence of music distraction upon reading efficiency. The Journal of Educational Research, 31, 264-271.
Freeburne, C. M., & Fleischer, M. S. (1952). The effect of music distraction upon reading rate and comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 43(2), 101-109. doi: 10.1037/h0054219
Friederici, A. D., Pfeifer, E., & Hahne, A. (1993). Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violations Cognitive Brain Research 1, 183-192.
Gordon, R. L., Schön, D., Magne, C., Astésano, C., & Besson, M. (2010). Words and Melody Are Intertwined in Perception of Sung Words: EEG and Behavioral Evidence. PLoS ONE, 5(3), e9889. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009889
Greenhouse, S., & Geisser, S. (1959). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika, 24(2), 95-112. doi: 10.1007/bf02289823
Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (2000). ERP effects of listening to speech: semantic ERP effects. Neuropsychologia 38, 1518-1530.
Henderson, M. T., Crews, A., & Barlow, J. (1945). A study of the effect of music distraction on reading efficiency. Journal of Applied Psychology, 29(4), 313-317. doi: 10.1037/h0056128
Henson, R. N. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of priming. Progress in neurobiology, 70(1),53-81.
Holcomb, P. J., & Anderson, J. E. (1993). Cross-modal semantic priming: A time-course analysis using event-related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 379-411. doi: 10.1080/01690969308407583
Holcomb, P. J., & Neville, H. J. (1991). Natural speech processing: An analysis using event-related brain potentials. Psychobiology, 19(4), 286-300.
James, C. T. (1975). The role of semantic information in lexical decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1(2), 130-136.
Koriat, A. (1981). Semantic facilitation in lexical decision as a function of prime-target association. Memory & Cognition, 9(6), 587-598. doi: 10.3758/bf03202353
Kounios, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (1994). Concreteness effects in semantic processing: ERP evidence supporting dual-coding theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 804-823.
Kutas, M. (1987). Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) elicited during rapid serial visual presentation of congruous and incongruous sentences. In J. R. Johnson, J. W. Rohrbaugh & R. Parasurman (Eds.), Current Trends in Event-related Potentials Research (pp. 406-411). New York: Elsevier Science Publishers B. V.
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Science 4, 463-470.
74
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty Years and Counting: Finding Meaning in the N400 Component of the Event-Related Brain Potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 621-647. doi: doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980a). Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words. Biological Psychology, 11(2), 99-116. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(80)90046-0
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980b). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203-205.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1982). The lateral distribution of event-related potentials during sentence processing. Neuropsychologia, 20(5), 579-590. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(82)90031-8
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984a). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307, 161-163.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984b). Word expectancy and event-related brain potentials during sentence processing. In S. Kornblum & J. Requin (Eds.), Preparatory states and processes (pp. 217-237). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1989). An Electrophysiological Probe of Incidental Semantic Association. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 38-49. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1989.1.1.38
75
Liu, B., Jin, Z., Wang, Z., & Xin, S. (2011). An ERP study on whether the P600 can reflect the presence of unexpected phonology. Experimental Brain Research 212, 399-408.
Luck, S. J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential technique: MIT Press.
Marton, M., & Szirtes, J. (1988). Saccade-related brain potentials during reading correct and incorrect versions of proverbs. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 6(4), 273-280. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(88)90014-1
Marton, M., Szirtes, J., & Breuer, P. (1984). The effect of stimulus probability on the late components of lambda responses in "oddball" and guessing task. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie mit Zeitschrift fur angewandte Psychologie(Suppl 6), 31-46.
Marton, M., Szirtes, J., & Breuer, P. (1985). Electrocortical signs of word categorization in saccade-related brain potentials and visual evoked potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 3(2), 131-144. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(85)90033-9
Marton, M., Szirtes, J., Donauer, N., & Breuer, P. (1985). Saccade-related brain potentials in semantic categorization tasks. Biological Psychology, 20(3), 163-184. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(85)90060-2
Näätänen, R., & Alho, K. (1997). Mismatch negativity--the measure for central sound representation accuracy. Audiology & Neurotology, 2(5), 341-353. doi: 10.1159/000259255
Neely, J. H., Keefe, D. E., & Ross, K. L. (1989). Semantic priming in the lexical decision task: Roles of prospective prime-generated expectancies and retrospective semantic matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1003-1019. doi: 10.1037/h0027577
Neville, H. J., Kutas, M., Chesney, G., & Schmidt, A. L. (1986). Event-related brain potentials during initial encoding and recognition memory of congruous and incongruous words. Journal of Memory and Language, 25(1), 75-92. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(86)90022-7
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97-113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(6), 785-806. doi: 10.1016/0749-596x(92)90039-z
Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P. J., & Swinney, D. A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 786-803. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.786
Osterhout, L., McLaughlin, J., & Bersick, M. (1997). Event-related brain potentials and human language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(6), 203-209. doi:
77
10.1016/s1364-6613(97)01073-5
Perea, M., & Rosa, E. (2002). The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task. Psychological Research, 66(3), 180-194. doi: 10.1007/s00426-002-0086-5
Rossell, S. L., Price, C. J., & Nobre, A. C. (2003). The anatomy and time course of semantic priming investigated by fMRI and ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 41(5), 550-564. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00181-1
Scharinger, M., & Felder, V. (2011). ERP signatures of cross-modal semantic fragment priming: Early context effects in speech perception. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 80(1), 19-27. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.01.006
SchÖN, D., Gordon, R. L., & Besson, M. (2005). Musical and Linguistic Processing in Song Perception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060(1), 71-81. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.006
Shtyrov, Y., Kujala, T., Palva, S., Ilmoniemi, R. J., & Näätänen, R. (2000). Discrimination of Speech and of Complex Nonspeech Sounds of Different Temporal Structure in the Left and Right Cerebral Hemispheres. NeuroImage, 12(6), 657-663. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0646
Wong, P. C. M., & Diehl, R. L. (2002). How Can the Lyrics of a Song in a Tone Language Be Understood? Psychology of Music, 30(2), 202-209. doi: 10.1177/0305735602302006