研究生: |
高立航 Kao, Li-Hang |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
同儕權威概念與命令類型對於五歲幼兒權威判斷之影響 The Effect of Peer Authority Concept and Types of Command on Five-Year-Old Children's Authority Judgment. |
指導教授: | 廖鳳瑞 |
學位類別: |
碩士 Master |
系所名稱: |
人類發展與家庭學系 Department of Human Development and Family Studies |
論文出版年: | 2013 |
畢業學年度: | 101 |
語文別: | 中文 |
論文頁數: | 116 |
中文關鍵詞: | 規則認知 、同儕權威概念 、社會知識 、領域理論 、幼兒 |
英文關鍵詞: | cognition of rules, peer authority concept, social knowledge, domain theory, chlidren |
論文種類: | 學術論文 |
相關次數: | 點閱:175 下載:11 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
本研究檢視在不同同儕權威概念及命令類型下,五歲幼兒的權威判斷情形。同儕權威概念包括具有社會地位特徵、知識與技巧特徵及幼兒個別特徵的同儕權威角色,命令類型包括道德性命令、中性命令及社會性命令。本研究採用個別訪談方式進行,研究樣本為149位新北市的五歲幼兒,施測的工具為自編的「同儕介入幼兒違規事件之情境故事」,施測的內容共分成三個部分,第一部分在測驗受試者對於具有幼兒個別特徵的同儕權威概念,第二部分在測驗受試者對不同同儕權威概念及不同命令類型之命令的合法性及服從性判斷,並對判斷的理由進行解釋,第三部分測驗在中性命令下,兩種具有不同同儕權威概念特徵的同儕給予相對立的命令時,受試者對於需要優先遵守何者命令的判斷,最後施測結果以二因子相依樣本變異數分析、卡方考驗進行統計分析,並將幼兒對於具有幼兒個別特徵的同儕權威概念及合法性與服從性判斷理由進行歸類。本研究的重要發現如下:
一、 五歲幼兒認為同儕權威概念包括社會地位特徵、知識與技巧特徵及幼兒個別特徵;具有幼兒個別特徵的同儕權威概念包括:外在特徵、內在特質、知識與專長、友誼關係四種類型,其中友誼關係是構成幼兒同儕權威概念的最重要因素。
二、 在道德性命令下,具有社會地位特徵的同儕最有合法性及服從性,而知識與技巧特徵則必須依賴社會地位特徵的存在才具有合法性及服從性。
三、 在中性命令下,具有社會地位特徵、知識與技巧特徵的同儕都具合法性及服從性,不過社會地位特徵的合法性及服從性大於知識與技巧特徵。
四、 在社會性命令下,幼兒對於合法性與服從性的判斷不一致,在合法性的判斷上,幼兒認為只有社會地位特徵才具有合法性,在服從性判斷上,幼兒的判斷情形與中性命令相似。
本研究根據上述結果建議未來研究可以針對本研究所發現具有幼兒個別特徵的權威概念進行探究,亦可以探究社會規範及情境因素對於幼兒同儕權威概念的影響;除此之外,研究對象亦可延伸至五歲以下或學齡階段的兒童。
This study examines the five-year-old children’s authority judgment with regard to different peer authority concepts and the types of command. The peer authority concepts consist of three authority attributes: social position, skill and knowledge, and personal authority attribute. The types of command include three types: moral, neutral, and social. This study used the method of individual interview. The subjects were 149 five-year-old children in New Taipei City. The tool used of the interview called ” the peers intervened the violation events”, which have three parts of assessment. First, it assessed their peer authority concepts from the peer who possessing personal authority attribute. Second, it assessed their evaluations of commands from different peer authority concepts and the types of command in judging legitimacy and obedience, and the rationale of judging. Third, it assessed their evaluations of commands from the peers who possessing different authority attributes and their choices between different peers who gave opposing commands in the neutral command. Finally, the data from the assessments were statistically analyzed by two-way ANOVA, and Chi-square. Besides, the peer authority concepts from the peer who possessing personal authority attribute and the rationale of judging were generalized. The major findings of this study were as follows:
1. The five-year-old children’s peer authority concepts include social position authority attribute, skill and knowledge authority attribute, and personal authority attribute. In addition, personal authority attribute include external figure, internal figure, skill and knowledge, and friendship. Especially, friendship is the most important element of peer authority concepts.
2. The subject weighted social position more heavily than skill and knowledge and personal authority attribute in judging legitimacy and obedience in the moral command. However, the subject weighted skill and knowledge more heavily than personal authority attribute in judging legitimacy and obedience when the peer both possessing social position.
3. The subject weighted social position more heavily than skill and knowledge in the neutral command, but they weighted both of them more heavily than personal authority attribute.
4. The subject used different ways of judging legitimacy and obedience in social command. They weighted social position more heavily than skill and knowledge personal authority attribute in judging legitimacy, on the other hand, they weighted skill and knowledge less heavily than social position but more heavily than personal authority attribute in judging obedience.
Suggestions were according to the results show above for future studies, which can investigate the peer authority concepts from the peer who possessing personal authority attribute, and the effect upon the social norm and social context. Besides, the subject can extend under five years old or above that age.
中文部分:
行政院主計處(2011)。平均每戶家庭收支按戶內人數分。行政院主計處全球資訊網。取自http://win.dgbas.gov.tw/fies/a11.asp?year=100
行政院主計處綜合統計處家庭收支科(2011)。台灣地區家庭收支調查。行政院主計處全球資訊網。取自http://win.dgbas.gov.tw/fies/quick100.asp
吳靜琳(1996)。一位大班轉學生對班級規則學習之研究(未出版碩士論文)。國立臺灣師範大學,台北市。
李文正和李長燦(2005)。幼兒園教師實施班級常規之個案研究--以一位大班教師為例。幼兒保育學刊,3,205-130。
保心怡(1988)。大班幼兒對幼稚園內規則的認知研究(未出版碩士論文)。國立臺灣師範大學,台北市。
單文經和汪履維(譯)(1986)。道德發展與教學(R. H. Hersh, D. P. Paolitto, & J. Reimer)。台北市:五南。(原著出版年:1983)
張鄭主青(2006)。收不清 理還亂--幼兒收拾常規探討(未出版碩士論文)。臺北市立教育大學,台北市。
外文部分:
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1963). Social learning and personality development. New York: Holt.
Bellah, R., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W., Swidler, A., & Tiption, S. (1985). Habits of the heart: Individualism and commitment in American life. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Damon, W. (1977). The social world of the child. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Hollos, M., Leis,P., & Turiel, E. (1986).Social reasoning in Ijo children and adolescents in Nigerrian communities. Journal of Cross-Culture Psychology, 17, 352-376.
Killen, M., McGlothlin, H., & Lee-Kim, J. (2002). Between individuals and culture: Individual’s evalutions of exclusion from social group. In H. Keller, Y. Poortinga, & A. Schoelmerich (Eds.), Between Biology and culture: Perspectives on ontogenetic development (pp. 159-190). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Kim, J., & Turiel, E. (1996). Korean and American children's concepts of adult and peer authority. Social Development, 5(3), 310-329.
Laupa, M., & Tse, P. (2005). Authority Concepts Among Children and Adolescents in the Island of Macao. Social Development, 14(4), 652-663.
Laupa, M., & Turiel, E. (1986). Children’s conceptions of adult and peer authority. Child Development, 57, 405-412.
Laupa, M., & Turiel, E. (1993). Children’s conceptions of authority and social context. Juornal of Educational Psychology, 85, 191-197.
Laupa(1986). Children’s conceptions of adult and peer authority. Child Development, 57, 405-412.
Laupa, M. (1991). Children's reasoning about three authority attributes: Adult status, knowledge, and social position. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 321-329.
Laupa, M. (1994).Who's in Charge? Preschool children's concepts of authority. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9(1), 1-17.
Laupa, M. (1995).Children’s reasoning about authority in home and school contexts. Social Development, 4(1), 1-16.
Nisan, M. (1987).Moral norms and social conventional: A cross-culture comparison. Development Psychology, 23, 719-725.
Nucci, L. P. (1981).The development of personal concepts: A domain distinct from moral or societal concepts. Child Development, 52, 114-121.
Nucci, L. P. (2001). Education in the moral domain. New York: Cambridge University.
Piaget, J. (1932/1968). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Thornberg, R. (2008).School children’s reasoning about school rules. Research Papers in Education. 23(1), 37-52.
Rohner, R. P., & Pettengill, S. M. (1985).Perceived parental acceptance-rejection and parental control among Korean adolescents. Child Development, 36, 524-528.
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957).Patterns of child rearing. Evanston, Ⅲ.: Row-Peterson.
Shantz, C. U. (1987).Conflicts between children. Child Development, 58, 283-305.
Skinner, B. F. (1971).Beyond freedom and dignity. Indianapolis, Ind.:Hackett Pub.
Smetana, J. G. (1981).Preschool children’s conceptions of moral and social rules. Children Developmental Psychology, 3, 211-226.
Smetana, J. G. (1988).Adolescents’ and parents’ conceptions of parental authority. Child Development, 59, 321-335.
Smetana, J. G. (1989).Toddlers' social interactions in the context of moral and conventional transgressions in the home. Developmental Psycology, 25(4), 499-508.
Smetana, J. G. (1995).Morality in context: Abstractions, ambiguities, and applications. In R. Vasta (ed.), Annals of Child Development, 10, 83-130.
Smetana, J. G., & Turiel, E. (2003).Morality during adolescence. In G. R. Adams & M. Berzonsky (Eds.), The blackwell handbook of adolescence (pp.247-268). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Song, M. J., Smetana, J. G., & Kim, S. Y. (1987). Korean children's conceptions of moral and conventional transgressions. Developmental Psychology, 23(4), 577-582.
Tisak, M. (1986).Child’s conception of parental authority. Child Development, 57, 166-176.
Triandis, H. C. (1990).Crosscultural studies of individualism and collectivism. In I. Berman (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Metivation (pp. 41-133). Luncoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Turiel, E. (1977).Distinct conceptual and developmental domains: Social convention and morality. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 25, 77-116.
Turiel(1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. New York: Cambridge University.
Turiel, E. (1978).The development of concepts of social structure: Social convention. In J. Glick & A. Clarke-Stewart (Eds.), The development of social understanding. New York: Gardner Press.
Weston, D., & Turiel, E.(1980). Act-rule relations:Children’s concepts of social rules. Developmental Psychology, 16, 417-424.
Yau, J. & Smetana, J. G. (2003). Conceptions of moral, social-conventional, and personal events among Chinese preschoolers in Hong Kong. Child Development, 74(3), 647-658.