簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 侯邰嫺
Hou, Tai-Hsien
論文名稱: 以情境化數位遊戲提升學生執行功能:抑制與轉換
Improving Students' Executive Functions by Situational Digital Games: Inhibition and Shifting
指導教授: 陳志洪
Chen, Zhi-Hong
口試委員: 陳學志
Chen, Hsueh-Chih
鄭年亨
Cheng, Nien-Heng
陳志洪
Chen, Zhi-Hong
口試日期: 2022/07/08
學位類別: 碩士
Master
系所名稱: 資訊教育研究所
Graduate Institute of Information and Computer Education
論文出版年: 2022
畢業學年度: 110
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 69
中文關鍵詞: 執行功能數位遊戲情境化
英文關鍵詞: executive functions, digital games, situational
研究方法: 準實驗設計法
DOI URL: http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202200867
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:100下載:18
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 人能有效地自我監控、彈性調整自身的認知行為與採取行動,都是由大腦處 理後的結果,而意識控制思想、行動的心理過程被稱為-“「執行功能」”,它能夠幫助實現目標導向的思想和行為,有效提升生活品質,在學業或事業上都有所幫助。執行功能中的抑制、轉換更被認為是協助高階執行功能的重要基礎,因此若能有效提升執行功能中的抑制及轉換能力,將能協助我們做出良好的決策。
    過去研究發現執行功能能透過後天訓練提升,常見訓練方式有:(1)運動訓練:傳統上最常使用的訓練模式,以有氧運動為主 (2)認知訓練:透過特定認知測驗訓練大腦,其中數位化訓練效果優於非數位化訓練 (3)遊戲式訓練:透過遊戲訓練認知能力,其中基於認知理論設計的遊戲最為有效。
    過往訓練結束後,受測者受到許多限制,導致無動機繼續訓練,訓練停止後 成效也逐漸消退,而遊戲式訓練雖能提升使用者內在動機,但在情境設計上多為虛構式環境,難使受測者將訓練成效遷移至日常生活之中,因此,本研究希望透過遊戲式的趣味性、有效性、方便性,提升繼續遊玩的意願,再加入生活的情境, 加強情境化的延續性、帶入性,提升成效及遷移,故設計情境化數位遊戲提升抑制及轉換。
    為評估上述情境化數位遊戲對執行功能的影響,因此本研究探討以下三個研 究問題:(1)情境化數位遊戲是否能提升執行功能中的抑制能力;(2)情境化數位遊戲是否能提升執行功能中的轉換能力;(3)情境化數位遊戲對玩家遊戲體驗的影響。
    研究對象為北部大學學生 58 人,隨機分為實驗組及控制組,實驗組透過情境化數位遊戲進行訓練,控制組則透過認知工具進行訓練。實驗流程先進行執行功能前測,之後訓練系統介入三十分鐘,並同步使用眼動儀追蹤使用者遊戲中的眼動歷程,最後進行執行功能後測、ARCS 問卷及訪談。透過使用者先天執行功能能力(前測)及介入後的執行功能能力(後測)差異了解訓練工具對使用者的影響,再藉由 ARCS 問卷(注意力、相關性、信心、滿足)、訪談及眼動儀了解對玩家遊戲體驗的影響。
    研究結果顯示:(1)抑制能力在情境化數位遊戲訓練後有顯著提升;(2) 轉換能 力在情境化數位遊戲訓練後有顯著提升;(3)分析玩家遊戲體驗的影響後,研究結果發現:情境化數位遊戲較能吸引使用者的注意力,讓使用者產生傾向積極的態度 並有偏向能建立成功的自信心,且較能讓使用者產生持續遊玩的想法,眼動指標表示規則說明、計分區、遊戲區、規則提示、按鈕選項皆有顯著被使用,且計分 區和規則提示等更能影響使用者,使其強化訓練效果,情境化的主題也有幫助玩家投入的效果,而控制組則發現文字與影像規則說明能幫助使用者更清楚如何操作。

    People can effectively self-monitor, flexibly adjust their cognitive behaviors and take actions. All of these are processed by the brain, and the psychological process of consciously controlling thoughts and actions is called "Executive Function". As to achieve goal-oriented, thoughts and behaviors can effectively improve the quality of life and help in academic or career. Inhibition and shifting in executive functions are considered to be an important basis for assisting higher-order executive functions. Therefore, if we can effectively improve the ability of inhibition and shifting in executive functions, it will help us make good decisions.
    Prior research has found that executive function can be improved through acquired training. Common training methods include: (1) Exercise training: the most commonly used training mode traditionally, mainly aerobic exercise (2) Cognitive training: training the brain through specific cognitive tests. Among them, the effect of digital training is better than that of non-digital training. (3) Game-based training: training cognitive ability through games, among which games based on cognitive theory are the most effective.
    In the past, the subjects face many restrictions after training, resulting in a lack of motivation to continue the training, so the effectiveness of training gradually fade after the training was stopped. Although game-based training can improve the user's intrinsic motivation, the situation is mostly in a fictional environment, which is difficult to transfer into real life. To enable the subjects to transfer the training effect to their daily life, this study aimed to enhance the willingness to continue playing through the fun, effectiveness, and convenience of the game, and add the context of everyday life to strengthen the continuity of situation, immersion, improving effectiveness and migration. Therefore, this study designed a situational digital game to improve inhibition and shifting.
    In order to evaluate the impact of the situational digital games on executive function mentioned above, this study explores the following three research questions: (1) Does the situational digital games improve the inhibitory ability (2) Does the situational digital games improve the ability of shifting (3) What are the influences of situational digital games design on players ' gaming experience.
    The research subjects were 58 college students from northern Taiwan, who were randomly divided into the experimental group and the control group. The experimental group was trained through situational digital games, and the control group was trained through cognitive training tools. The experimental process first conduct a pre-test of executive function, and the training system was involved for 30 minutes, which the eye tracker was used to track the user's eye movement in the game. Finally, the post-test of executive function, ARCS questionnaire, and an interview was conducted. To reveal the impact of the digital game, the users’ innate executive function ability (pre-test), post-intervention executive function ability (post-test), ARCS questionnaire (attention, relevance, confidence, satisfaction), interview, and eye tracking data are collected.
    The results of the study show that: (1) the inhibition ability is significantly improved after the situational digital games training ; (2) the shifting ability is significantly improved after the situational digital games training ; (3) after analyzing the impact of players' gaming experience, the results revealed that: situational digital games can attract users' attention, keep users a positive attitude, develop self-confidence, and make users have the idea of ''continue to play” than before. Eye movement indicators indicate rule descriptions, scoring areas, game areas, rule prompts, and button options are all significantly used. Besides, the scoring zones and rule prompts can affect users and enhance training effects. To sum up, the situational themes assist players to engage in the game, while the control group found that texts and the video rules description can help users understand how to operate the whole process more clearly.

    摘要 I ABSTRACT III 致謝 V 第一章 緒論 1 第一節 研究背景 1 第二節 研究目的 3 第三節 名詞釋義 4 第二章 文獻探討 5 第一節 執行功能 5 一、 前額葉 5 二、 基礎執行功能能力 8 三、 執行功能優劣影響 10 四、 執行功能訓練方式 11 1. 運動訓練 11 2. 認知訓練 12 3. 遊戲式訓練 13 五、 國內執行功能過往研究 14 第二節 數位遊戲之應用 17 第三節 情境化之應用 18 第三章 系統設計 21 第一節 系統介面 21 一、 遊戲設計 21 二、 投球模式 22 三、 打擊模式 24 第四章 研究方法 27 第一節 研究設計 27 第二節 研究對象 27 第三節 研究工具 28 一、 腦力運動會 28 二、 Simon 28 三、 執行功能測驗 29 四、 ARCS問卷 31 五、 Tobii眼動儀 32 六、 訪談 34 第四節 資料分析 36 第五章 結果與討論 37 第一節 抑制成效 37 第二節 轉換成效 38 第三節 ARCS成效 39 第四節 訪談及眼動歷程分析結果 40 一、 訪談結果分析 40 1. 實驗組 40 2. 控制組 43 二、 眼動數據分析 45 1. 實驗組 45 2. 控制組 48 三、 眼動序列分析 49 1. 實驗組 49 2. 控制組 54 第六章 結論與建議 56 第一節 研究結論 56 第二節 研究限制 58 一、 樣本限制 58 二、 研究設計 58 參考文獻 59 中文部分 59 英文部分 59 附錄一 ARCS問卷 67

    中文部分
    吳武典. (1997)。 國中偏差行為學生學校生活適應之探討。 教育心理學報, 訝, 25, 49.
    洪聰敏.(2021)。 原來大腦可以這樣練:提升學習抗壓力,成功者的大腦運動訓練課程。臺北市:時報出版。

    英文部分
    Abdul Jabbar, A. I., & Felicia, P. (2015). Gameplay engagement and learning in game-based learning: A systematic review. Review of educational research, 85(4), 740-779.
    Alvarez, J. A., & Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology review, 16(1), 17-42.
    AL‐Ayash, A., Kane, R. T., Smith, D., & Green‐Armytage, P. (2016). The influence of color on student emotion, heart rate, and performance in learning environments. Color Research & Application, 41(2), 196-205.
    Anguera, J., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 501, 97–101 (2013).
    Arnsten, A. F. (1998). The biology of being frazzled. Science, 280(5370), 1711-1712.
    Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child development, 81(6), 1641-1660.
    Best, J. R. (2010). Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: Contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Developmental review, 30(4), 331-351.
    Bediou, B., Adams, D. M., Mayer, R. E., Tipton, E., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2018). Meta-analysis of action video game impact on perceptual, attentional, and cognitive skills. Psychological bulletin, 144(1), 77.
    Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). Individual development and evolution: experiential canalization of self-regulation. Developmental psychology, 48(3), 647.
    Blair, C. (2016). Developmental science and executive function. Current directions in psychological science, 25(1), 3-7.
    Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2008). The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta psychologica, 129(3), 387-398.
    Boot, W. R., & Simons, D. J. (2012). Advances in video game methods and reporting practices (but still room for improvement): A commentary on Strobach, Frensch, and Schubert (2012). Acta psychologica, 141(2), 276-277.
    Cicerone, K., Levin, H., Malec, J., Stuss, D., & Whyte, J. (2006). Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for executive function: moving from bench to bedside in patients with traumatic brain injury. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 18(7), 1212-1222.
    Chan, R. C., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T., & Chen, E. Y. (2008). Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Archives of clinical neuropsychology, 23(2), 201-216.
    Chang, Y. K., Huang, C. J., Chen, K. F., & Hung, T. M. (2013). Physical activity and working memory in healthy older adults: an ERP study. Psychophysiology, 50(11), 1174-1182.
    Clark, D. B., Tanner-Smith, E. E., & Killingsworth, S. S. (2016). Digital games, design, and learning: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Review of educational research, 86(1), 79-122.
    Clark, R. C., & Mayer, R. E. (2016). E-learning and the science of instruction: Proven guidelines for consumers and designers of multimedia learning. john Wiley & sons.
    Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., McAuley, E., Erickson, K. I., & Scalf, P. (2004). Neurocognitive aging and cardiovascular fitness. Journal of molecular neuroscience, 24(1), 9-14.
    Dawson, P., & Guare, R. (2012). Coaching students with executive skills deficits. Guilford Press.
    Diamond, A. (2005). Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (with hyperactivity). Development and psychopathology, 17(3), 807-825.
    Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959-964.
    Djabelkhir, L., Wu, Y. H., Vidal, J. S., Cristancho-Lacroix, V., Marlats, F., Lenoir, H., ... & Rigaud, A. S. (2017). Computerized cognitive stimulation and engagement programs in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: comparing feasibility, acceptability, and cognitive and psychosocial effects. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 12, 1967.
    Eakin, L., Minde, K., Hechtman, L., Ochs, E., Krane, E., Bouffard, R., Greenfield, B., & Looper, K. (2004). The marital and family functioning of adults with ADHD and their spouses. Journal of attention disorders, 8(1), 1–10.
    Fairchild, G., van Goozen, S. H., Stollery, S. J., Aitken, M. R., Savage, J., Moore, S. C., & Goodyer, I. M. (2009). Decision making and executive function in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder and control subjects. Biological psychiatry, 66(2), 162-168.
    Gates, N., & Valenzuela, M. (2010). Cognitive exercise and its role in cognitive function in older adults. Current psychiatry reports, 12(1), 20-27.
    Gao, X. P., & Xin, J. H. (2006). Investigation of human's emotional responses on colors. Color Research & Application: Endorsed by Inter‐Society Color Council, The Colour Group (Great Britain), Canadian Society for Color, Color Science Association of Japan, Dutch Society for the Study of Color, The Swedish Colour Centre Foundation, Colour Society of Australia, Centre Français de la Couleur, 31(5), 411-417.
    Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational psychologist, 41(2), 111-127.
    Homer, B. D., Plass, J. L., Raffaele, C., Ober, T. M., & Ali, A. (2018). Improving high school
    Homer, B. D., Ober, T. M., Rose, M. C., MacNamara, A., Mayer, R. E., & Plass, J. L. (2019). Speed versus accuracy: Implications of adolescents' neurocognitive developments in a digital game to train executive functions. Mind, Brain, and Education, 13(1), 41-52.
    Homer, B. D., Plass, J. L., Rose, M. C., MacNamara, A. P., Pawar, S., & Ober, T. M. (2019). Activating adolescents’“hot” executive functions in a digital game to train cognitive skills: The effects of age and prior abilities. Cognitive Development, 49, 20-32.
    Hongpaisanwiwat, C., & Lewis, M. (2003). Attentional effect of animated character. Proceedings of the human-computer interaction. iOS Press, 423-30.
    Jak, A. J., Seelye, A. M., & Jurick, S. M. (2013). Crosswords to computers: a critical review of popular approaches to cognitive enhancement. Neuropsychology review, 23(1), 13-26.
    Kerr, A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2004). Development of “hot” executive function: The children’s gambling task. Brain and cognition, 55(1), 148-157.
    Keller, J. M. (1987). Development and use of the ARCS model of instructional design. Journal of instructional development, 10(3), 2-10.
    Kern, H., & Schumann, M. (1987). Limits of the division of labour. New production and employment concepts in West German industry. Economic and Industrial Democracy, 8(2), 151-170.
    Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in cognitive sciences, 14(7), 317-324.
    Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Cohen, N. J., Banich, M. T., McAuley, E., Harrison, C. R., Chason, J., Vakil, E., Bardell, L., Boileau, R. A., & Colcombe, A. (1999). Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature, 400(6743), 418–419.
    Kramer, A. F., & Erickson, K. I. (2007). Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(8), 342-348.
    Kurz, A. F., Leucht, S., & Lautenschlager, N. T. (2011). The clinical significance of cognition-focused interventions for cognitively impaired older adults: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. International Psychogeriatrics, 23(9), 1364-1375.
    Kueider, A. M., Parisi, J. M., Gross, A. L., & Rebok, G. W. (2012). Computerized cognitive training with older adults: a systematic review. PloS one, 7(7), e40588.
    Lubart, T. I., & Getz, I. (1997). Emotion, metaphor, and the creative process. Creativity research journal, 10(4), 285-301.
    Lui, M., & Tannock, R. (2007). Working memory and inattentive behaviour in a community sample of children. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(1), 1-11.
    McAuley, E., Mullen, S.P., Szabo, A.N., White, S.M., Wójcicki, T.R., Mailey, E.L., Gothe, N.P., Olson, E.A., Voss, M.W., Erickson, K.I., Prakash, R.S., & Kramer, A.F. (2011). Self-regulatory processes and exercise adherence in older adults: executive function and self-efficacy effects. American journal of preventive medicine, 41 3, 284-90 .
    Manjunath, N. K., & Telles, S. (2001). Improved performance in the Tower of London test following yoga. Indian journal of physiology and pharmacology, 45(3), 351-354.
    Mayer, R. E., & Estrella, G. (2014). Benefits of emotional design in multimedia instruction. Learning and Instruction, 33, 12-18.
    Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual review of neuroscience, 24(1), 167-202.
    Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive psychology, 41(1), 49-100.
    Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. (2007). Interactive multimodal learning environments. Educational psychology review, 19(3), 309-326.
    Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., ... & Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the national Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2693-2698.
    Monsell, S. (2021). Control of mental processes. In Unsolved mysteries of the mind (pp. 93-148). Psychology Press.
    Mozaffari, M., Hassani-Abharian, P., Kholghi, G., Vaseghi, S., Zarrindast, M. R., & Nasehi, M. (2022). Treatment with RehaCom computerized rehabilitation program improves response control, but not attention in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 98, 149-153.
    Ninaus, M., Greipl, S., Kiili, K., Lindstedt, A., Huber, S., Klein, E., ... & Moeller, K. (2019). Increased emotional engagement in game-based learning–A machine learning approach on facial emotion detection data. Computers & Education, 142, 103641.
    Nouchi, R., Taki, Y., Takeuchi, H., Hashizume, H., Akitsuki, Y., Shigemune, Y., Sekiguchi, A., Kotozaki, Y., Tsukiura, T., Yomogida, Y., & Kawashima, R. (2012). Brain training game improves executive functions and processing speed in the elderly: a randomized controlled trial. PloS one, 7(1), e29676.
    Ober, T. M., Brenner, C. J., Olsen, A., Homer, B. D., & Plass, J. L. (2021). Detecting patterns of engagement in a digital cognitive skills training game. Computers & Education, 165, 104144.
    Parong, J., Mayer, R. E., Fiorella, L., MacNamara, A., Homer, B. D., & Plass, J. L. (2017). Learning executive function skills by playing focused video games. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51, 141-151.
    Parong, J., & Mayer, R. E. (2020). Cognitive consequences of playing brain‐training games in immersive virtual reality. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34(1), 29-38.
    Penades, R., Catalan, R., Rubia, K., Andres, S., Salamero, M., & Gasto, C. (2007). Impaired response inhibition in obsessive compulsive disorder. European Psychiatry, 22(6), 404-410.
    Picard, R. W., & Klein, J. (2002). Computers that recognise and respond to user emotion: theoretical and practical implications. Interacting with computers, 14(2), 141-169.
    Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M. D., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. Journal of experimental child psychology, 108(3), 621-637.
    Prince, M., Patel, V., Saxena, S., Maj, M., Maselko, J., Phillips, M. R., & Rahman, A. (2007). No health without mental health. The lancet, 370(9590), 859-877.
    Plass, J. L., Heidig, S., Hayward, E. O., Homer, B. D., & Um, E. (2014). Emotional design in multimedia learning: Effects of shape and color on affect and learning. Learning and Instruction, 29, 128-140.
    Plass, J. L., Homer, B. D., & Kinzer, C. K. (2015). Foundations of game-based learning. Educational psychologist, 50(4), 258-283.
    Plass, J. L., & Kaplan, U. (2016). Emotional design in digital media for learning. In Emotions, technology, design, and learning (pp. 131-161). Academic Press.
    Plass, J. L., & Kalyuga, S. (2019). Four ways of considering emotion in cognitive load theory. Educational Psychology Review, 31(2), 339-359.
    Plass, J.L., Homer, B.D., Macnamara, A., Ober, T.M., Rose, M.C., Pawar, S., Hovey, C.M., & Olsen, A. (2019). Emotional Design for Digital Games for Learning: The Affective Quality of Expression, Color, Shape, and Dimensionality of Game Characters.
    Riggs, N. R., Spruijt-Metz, D., Chou, C. P., & Pentz, M. A. (2012). Relationships between executive cognitive function and lifetime substance use and obesity-related behaviors in fourth grade youth. Child Neuropsychology, 18(1), 1-11.
    Rogers, L. (2011). Developing simulations in multi-user virtual environments to enhance hralthcare education. British Journal of Educational Technology, 42(4), 608-615
    Rotgans, J. I., & Schmidt, H. G. (2011). Situational interest and academic achievement in the active-learning classroom. Learning and Instruction, 21(1), 58-67.
    Schwartz, R. N., & Plass, J. L. (2020). Types of engagement in learning with games.
    Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199-209.
    Schiavella, M., Pelagatti, M., Westin, J., Lepore, G., & Cherubini, P. (2018). Profiling online poker players: are executive functions correlated with poker ability and problem gambling?. Journal of gambling studies, 34(3), 823-851.
    Shute, V. J. (2011). Stealth assessment in computer-based games to support learning. Computer games and instruction, 55(2), 503-524.
    Shute, V. J., & Kim, Y. J. (2014). Formative and stealth assessment. In Handbook of research on educational communications and technology (pp. 311-321). Springer, New York, NY.
    Shute, V. J., & Rahimi, S. (2017). Review of computer‐based assessment for learning in elementary and secondary education. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 33(1), 1-19.
    Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2016). Do “brain-training” programs work?. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17(3), 103-186.
    Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive psychology, 33(1), 5-42.
    Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity. American psychologist, 51(7), 677.
    Sternberg, R. J. (1997). The concept of intelligence and its role in lifelong learning and success. American psychologist, 52(10), 1030.
    Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Culture and intelligence. American psychologist, 59(5), 325.
    Strobach, T., Frensch, P. A., & Schubert, T. (2012). Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. Acta psychologica, 140(1), 13-24.
    Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1984). Neuropsychological studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological bulletin, 95(1), 3.
    Stuss, D. T., & Levine, B. (2002). Adult clinical neuropsychology: lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Annual review of psychology, 53(1), 401-433.
    Stuss, D. T., & Anderson, V. (2004). The frontal lobes and theory of mind: Developmental concepts from adult focal lesion research. Brain and cognition, 55(1), 69-83.
    Suchman, L. A. (1987). Plans and situated actions: The problem of human-machine communication. Cambridge university press.
    Tekin, S., & Cummings, J. L. (2002). Frontal–subcortical neuronal circuits and clinical neuropsychiatry: an update. Journal of psychosomatic research, 53(2), 647-654.
    Um, E., Plass, J. L., Hayward, E. O., & Homer, B. D. (2012). Emotional design in multimedia learning. Journal of educational psychology, 104(2), 485.
    Van der Kleij, F. M., Feskens, R. C., & Eggen, T. J. (2015). Effects of feedback in a computer-based learning environment on students’ learning outcomes: A meta-analysis. Review of educational research, 85(4), 475-511.
    van Uffelen, J. G., Paw, M. J. C. A., Hopman-Rock, M., & van Mechelen, W. (2008). The effects of exercise on cognition in older adults with and without cognitive decline: a systematic review. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 18(6), 486-500.
    Wells, A., Mayer, R. E., Plass, J. L., & Homer, B. D. (2021). Playing a Video Game and Learning to Think: What’s the Connection?. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 5(4), 459-467.
    Whitlock, L. A., McLaughlin, A. C., & Allaire, J. C. (2012). Individual differences in response to cognitive training: Using a multi-modal, attentionally demanding game-based intervention for older adults. Computers in human behavior, 28(4), 1091-1096.
    Will Crescioni, A., Ehrlinger, J., Alquist, J. L., Conlon, K. E., Baumeister, R. F., Schatschneider, C., & Dutton, G. R. (2011). High trait self-control predicts positive health behaviors and success in weight loss. Journal of health psychology, 16(5), 750-759.
    Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child development perspectives, 6(4), 354-360.

    下載圖示
    QR CODE