研究生: |
湯鳴遠 Tang, Ming-Yuan |
---|---|
論文名稱: |
暴露時機、探索方式、與工作難度在節奏性動作表現之效應 The effect of exposure timing, exploratory mode, and task difficulty on rhythmic motor performance |
指導教授: |
楊梓楣
Yang, Chih-Mei |
學位類別: |
博士 Doctor |
系所名稱: |
體育學系 Department of Physical Education |
論文出版年: | 2017 |
畢業學年度: | 105 |
語文別: | 中文 |
論文頁數: | 100 |
中文關鍵詞: | 生態心理學 、知覺與行動 、動作變異性 |
英文關鍵詞: | ecological psychology, perception and action, movement variability |
DOI URL: | https://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202202400 |
論文種類: | 學術論文 |
相關次數: | 點閱:93 下載:10 |
分享至: |
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報 |
生態心理學強調環境與個體間的相互關係,個體暴露在環境中,透過探索覺知其與環境間之特定關係,進而產生適配的行為,且可能因工作難度導致不同的行為結果。本研究欲探知暴露時機、探索方式、與工作難度如何影響個體的踩踏表現。方法:招募 60 位成人為參與者,隨機分派至前暴行動組、前暴無行動組、後暴行動組、後暴無行動組、及等待控制組。參與者以兩種方式探索環境,分別為:一、觀察遊戲畫面及觸壓按鍵來因應工作。二、僅觀察遊戲畫面。透過 6 台紅外線攝影機收取運動學資料,並檢視參與者執行節奏性動作之時宜準確性、動作變異性、及進步幅度之情形。結果:(1). 在較佳表現時,觸壓按鍵探索相較於觀察探索,會有較準確的踩踏時宜。(2). 參與者暴露於遊戲情境,採取觸壓按鍵與觀察的探索,會有較大的動作變異性。(3). 工作難度由高至低相較於由低至高,得分之進步幅度較大。結論:首先,行動的探索使個體獲取較充沛的訊息,因而在較佳表現時有相對準確的踩踏時宜。再者,當個體暴露於遊戲情境中,同時採取探索活動,可促使其產生適應性的踩踏動作。最後,工作難度由高至低,因熱身減低與心理效應產生較大的進步幅度。
Ecological psychology emphasizes the mutual relations between individuals and their environment. While individuals are exposed to the environment, they can perceive the specific relationship between them and their environment, and produce an adaptive behavior through exploration. Moreover, the task difficulty may bring individuals generating different behaviors. This study aims to examine how the exposure timing, exploratory mode, and task difficulty affected individuals’ step performances. Method: We recruited 60 adults as participants. They were assigned randomly to pre-exposure action group, pre-exposure inaction group, re-exposure action group, re-exposure inaction group, and control group. Participants explored the environment in two modes. First, participants observed the image on the screen and pressed the button to cope with the task. Second, participants only observed the image on the screen. We used six infrared ray cameras to collect kinematic parameters and analyzed the timing accuracy, movement variability, and degree of progress on the rhythmic motor performance. Results: (1) As participants performed the relative good outcome, the button pressing exploration caused them acting better timing accuracy than the observing exploration. (2) When participants were exposed to the game situation, the observing exploration and button pressing exploration resulted in higher movement variability. (3) The task difficulty from high to low caused a greater degree of progress comparing to the task difficulty from low to high. Conclusion: While individuals execute better performance, the acting exploration makes them perceive more information to enhance timing accuracy. Furthermore, individuals yield adaptive step performances as they are exposed to game context and conduct exploratory actives. Finally, because of the warm-up decrement and psychological effect, the task difficulty from high to low brings in greater degrees of progress.
方正銘、卓俊伶 (2008)。動作表現與學習的帶寬回饋及年齡效應。臺灣運動心理學報,13,21-38。
史佳立 (2014)。視覺增益對施力變異性及運動單元活化模式的影響 (未出版博士論文)。國立成功大學,台南市。
江函芸、劉有德 (2007)。運動中的變異。中華體育季刊,21,42-52。
吳清山、林天祐 (2005)。教育新辭書。臺北市:高等教育。
李永碧 (譯) (2004)。體適能與全人健康的理論與實務。臺北市:藝軒圖書。(Werner, W. K., & Sharon, A, 1999)
林尚武、卓俊伶、陳重佑、楊梓楣 (2010)。工作難度對序列動作編序的影響及其年齡差異。體育學報,43,39-56。
邱皓政 (2010)。量化研究與統計分析:SPSS/PASW 資料分析範例解析(第五版)。台北:五南。
姚璠 (2001)。目標難度, 工作難度與回饋方式對國小學童排球發球表現與內在動機之影響。體育學報,31,137-148。
張春興 (1989)。張氏心學理辭典。台北:東華。
郭丁良、楊梓楣 (2010)。棒球揮擊時宜與球種判斷準確性之知覺-行動配連。體育學報,43,79-90。
陳秀惠 (2003)。運動學習曲線的意義¬—以跳舞機運動為例。台東師院學報,14,1-17。
雲端資料分析暨導引系統 (民 106 年 7 月 1 日)。單一樣本 t 檢定。取自
http://www.r-web.com.tw/stat/step1.php?method=one_sample_t_test
黃金柱、王俊明、劉碧華、王建臺、卓俊伶、林晉榮、楊宗文、石明宗、鄭景峰、林詠能、王育文、黃小玲 (譯) (2009)。體育研究法。臺北市:師大書苑。(Thomas, J. R., Nelson J. K., & Sliverman S. J., 1985)
黃嘉君、楊梓楣 (2008)。節奏性動作之影響因素。中華體育季刊,22,41-49。
楊梓楣、黃嘉笙、黃嘉君、劉盈君 (2010)。動靜態情境下環境賦使知覺過程之身體擺動與行動。大專體育學刊,12,20-28。
葉俊良、卓俊伶、林靜兒、陳重佑 (2007)。自我控制回饋對動作表現、學習及錯誤估計的效應。大專體育學刊,9,23-35。
廖庭儀 (2002)。自然回饋強度對運動技能學習曲線結構之影響(未出版之碩士論文)。國立臺灣師範大學,台北市。
維基百科 (民 105 年 4 月 20 日)。節奏【線上論壇】。取自 https://zh.wikipedia.org/wiki/节奏。
劉懿瑱、尹依萍、黃靖雅、張彩玲、林嘉玲 (2011)。縮短糖尿病共同照護網病人門診等候時間。臺灣膳食營養學雜誌,3,77-88。
劍橋線上字典 (民 105 年 6 月 13 日)。Exposure。取自
http://dictionary.cambridge.org/zht /詞典/英語-漢語-繁體/exposure_1
賴世炯、卓俊伶 (2000)。力量變異對飛鏢投擲動作表現的影響。體育學報,29,169-177。
簡曜輝、季力康、卓俊伶、 洪聰敏、黃英哲、黃崇儒、廖主民、盧俊宏 (譯) (2005)。競技與健身運動心理學。台北市:臺灣運動心理學會。(Weinberg, R. S., & Gould, D., 1999)
嚴雅婷、劉有德 (2008)。工作難度對學習平衡的影響。大專體育學刊,10,87-97。
Ammons, R. B. (1947). Acquisition of motor skill: I. Quantitative analysis and theoretical formulation. Psychological Review, 54, 263-281.
Adolph, K. E., Cole, W. G., Komati, M., Garciaguirre, J. S., Badaly, D., Lingeman, J. M., Chan, G. L. Y., Sotsky, R. B. (2012). How do you learn to walk? Thousands of steps and dozens of falls per day. Psychological Science, 23, 1387-1394.
Adolph, K. E., Eppler, M. A., & Gibson, E. J. (1993). Crawling versus walking infants' perception of affordances for locomotion over sloping surfaces. Child Development, 64, 1158-1174.
Arutyunyan, G. A., Gurfinkel, V. S., & Mirskii, M. L. (1968). Investigation of aiming at a target. Biophysics, 13, 536-538.
Arutyunyan, G. A., Gurfinkel, V. S., & Mirskii, M. L. (1969). Organization of movements on execution by man of an exact postural task. Biophysics, 14, 1162-1167.
Abernethy, B., Wood, J. M., & Parks, S. (1999). Can the anticipatory skills of experts be learned by novices? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 70, 313-318.
Barrow, H. M. (1954). Test of motor ability for college men. Research Quarterly. American Association for Health, Physical Education and Recreation, 25, 253-260.
Bernstein, N. (1967). The co-ordination and regulation of movement. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.
Bootsma, R. J. (1989). Accuracy of perceptual processes subserving different perception-action systems. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 489-500.
Caron, A. J., Caron, R. F., Caldwell, R. C., & Weiss, S. J. (1973). Infant perception of the structural properties of the face. Developmental Psychology, 9, 385-399.
Cole, W. G., Chan, G. L., Vereijken, B., & Adolph, K. E. (2013). Perceiving affordances for different motor skills. Experimental Brain Research, 225, 309-319.
Carello, C., Grosofsky, A., Reichel, F. D., Solomon, H. Y., & Turvey, M. T. (1989). Visually perceiving what is reachable. Ecological psychology, 1, 27-54.
Chuang, L. Y., Huang, C. J., & Hung, T. M. (2013). The differences in frontal midline theta power between successful and unsuccessful basketball free throws of elite basketball players. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 90, 321-328.
Cook, T. D., & Insko, C. A. (1968). Persistence of attitude change as a function of conclusion reexposure: A laboratory-field experiment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 322-328.
Donohue, K. C., & Spencer, R. M. (2011). Continuous re-exposure to environmental sound cues during sleep does not improve memory for semantically unrelated word pairs. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 10, 167-177.
Day, B. M., Wagman, J. B., & Smith, P. J. (2015). Perception of maximum stepping and leaping distance: Stepping affordances as a special case of leaping affordances. Acta Psychologica, 158, 26-35.
Elliott, S. A., & Brown, J. S. L. (2002). What are we doing to waiting list controls? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1047-1052.
Farrow, D., & Abernethy, B. (2002). Can anticipatory skills be learned through implicit video based perceptual training? Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 471-485.
Fink, P. W., Kelso, J. A., Jirsa, V. K., & De Guzman, G. (2000). Recruitment of degrees of freedom stabilizes coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 671-692.
Farrow, D., & Abernethy, B. (2003). Do expertise and the degree of perception-action coupling affect natural anticipatory performance? Perception, 32, 1127-1139.
Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gibson, J. J., & Gibson, E. J. (1955). Perceptual learning: Differentiation or enrichment? Psychological Review, 62, 32-41.
Guadagnoli, M. A., & Lee, T. D. (2004). Challenge point: A framework for conceptualizing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 212-224.
Goode, S. L., Meeuwsen, H. J., & Goggin, N. L. (1995). Coincidence-anticipation timing. Women in Sport and Physical Activity Journal, 4, 59-75.
Hill, T., Lewicki, P., Czyzewska, M., & Schuller, G. (1990). The role of learned inferential encoding rules in the perception of faces: Effects of nonconscious self-perpetuation of a bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 350-371.
Haken, H., Peper, C. E., Beek, P. J., & Daffertshofer, A. (1996). A model for phase transitions in human hand movements during multifrequency tapping. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 90, 179-196.
Hagemann, N., Strauss, B., & Cañal-Bruland, R. (2006). Training perceptual skill by orienting visual attention. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 28, 143-158.
Higuchi, T., Takada, H., Matsuura, Y., & Imanaka, K. (2004). Visual estimation of spatial requirements for locomotion in novice wheelchair users. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 55-66.
Irion, A. L. (1948). The relation of “set” to retention. Psychological Review, 55, 336-341.
Jackson, R. C., & Farrow, D. (2005). Implicit perceptual training: How, when, and why? Human Movement Science, 24, 308-325.
Kelso, J. A. (1984). Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 246, 1000-1004.
Kroll, W. P. (1971). Perspectives in physical education. New York: Academic Press.
Kim, S., Carlton, L. G., Liu, Y. T., & Newell, K. M. (1999). Impulse and movement space-time variability. Journal of Motor Behavior, 31, 341-357.
Kugler, P. N., Kelso, J. S., & Turvey, M. T. (1980). On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: I. Theoretical lines of convergence. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 3-47.). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Kelso, J. A. S., Scholz, J. P., & Schöner, G. (1986). Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: Critical fluctuations. Physics Letters A, 118, 279-284.
Loze, G. M., Collins, D., & Holmes, P. (2001). Pre-shot EEG alpha-power reactivity during expert air-pistol shooting: A comparison of best and worst shots. Journal of Sports Science, 19, 727-733.
Lee, T. D., Eliasz, K. L., Gonzalez, D., Alguire, K., Ding, K., & Dhaliwal, C. (2016). On the role of error in motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 48, 1-17.
Lavis, Y., & Mitchell, C. (2006). Effects of preexposure on stimulus discrimination: An investigation of the mechanisms responsible for human perceptual learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 2083-2101.
Liu, Y. T., Mayer-Kress, G., & Newell, K. M. (2006). Qualitative and quantitative in the dynamics of motor learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 380-393.
Moore, J. (1980). Neuroanatomical considerations relating to recovery of function following brain injury. In P. Bach-y-Rita (Ed.), Recovery of Function: Theoretical Consideration for Brain Injury Rehabilitation (pp.9-90). Baltimore: University Park.
Mark, L. S. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 361-370.
Mann, D. L., Abernethy, B., & Farrow, D. (2010). Action specificity increases anticipatory performance and the expert advantage in natural interceptive tasks. Acta Psychologica, 135, 17-23.
Mark, L. S., Balliett, J. A., Craver, K. D., Douglas, S. D., & Fox, T. (1990). What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for sitting. Ecological Psychology, 2, 325-366.
Mundy, M. E., Honey, R. C., & Dwyer, D. M. (2007). Simultaneous presentation of similar stimuli produces perceptual learning in human picture processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 124-138.
Müller, H., & Loosch, E. (1999). Functional variability and an equifinal path of movement during targeted throwing. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 36, 103-126.
Newell, K. M. (1986). Constraints on the development of coordination. In M. G. Wade & H. T. A. Whiting (Eds.), Motor development in children: Aspects of coordination and control (pp. 341-360). Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.
Newell, K. M., & Slifkin, A. B. (1998). The nature of movement variability. In J. P. Piek (Ed.), Motor behavior and human skill: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 143-160). United States of America: Human Kinetics.
Oudejans, R. R. D., Michaels, C. F., Bakker, F. C., & Dolne, M. A. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of catchableness of fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 879-891.
Peper, C. E., Beek, P. J., & van Wieringen, P. C. (1995). Multifrequency coordination in bimanual tapping: Asymmetrical coupling and signs of supercriticality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1117-1138.
Pepping, C. J., & Li, F. X. (2005). Effects of response task on reaction time and the detection of affordances. Motor Control, 9, 129-143.
Péruch, P., Vercher, J. L., & Gauthier, G. M. (1995). Acquisition of spatial knowledge through visual exploration of simulated environments. Ecological Psychology, 7, 1-20.
Romero, V., Coey, C., Schmidt, R. C., & Richardson, M. J. (2012). Movement coordination or movement interference: Visual tracking and spontaneous coordination modulate rhythmic movement interference. PLOS ONE, 7, 1-9.
Roskos, K., & Burstein, K. (2012). Descriptive observations of e-book shared reading at preschool. Journal of Literacy and Technology, 13, 27-57.
Rowe, M. K., & Craske, M. G. (1998). Effects of varied-stimulus exposure training on fear reduction and return of fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 719-734.
Ranganathan, R., & Carlton, L. G. (2007). Perception-action coupling and anticipatory performance in baseball batting. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 369-380.
Richardson, M. J., Campbell, W. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2009). Movement interference during action observation as emergent coordination. Neuroscience Letters, 448, 117-122.
Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Watson, T. L., Clifford, C. W., & Nakayama, K. (2003). Fitting the mind to the world face adaptation and attractiveness aftereffects. Psychological Science, 14, 558-566.
Ramsey, J. L., Langlois, J. H., Hoss, R. A., Rubenstein, A. J., & Griffin, A. M. (2004). Origins of a stereotype: Categorization of facial attractiveness by 6-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 7, 201-211.
Repp, B. H., & Penel, A. (2004). Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychological Research, 68, 252-270.
Ramenzoni, V. C., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2014). Scaling up perception–action links: Evidence from synchronization with individual and joint action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1551-1565.
Roskos, K. A., Sullivan, S., Simpson, D., & Zuzolo, N. (2016). E-books in the early literacy environment: Is there added value for vocabulary development? Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 30, 226-236.
Stahl, S. A. (2003). Words are learned incrementally over multiple exposures. American Educator, 27, 18-22.
Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Dekker, L. D., Vermeer, A., & Hopkins, B. (1998). Locomoting through apertures of different width: A study of children with cerebral palsy. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 2, 5-13.
Schimidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (2014). Motor learning and performance: From principles to practice (5th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Shannon, C. E., & Weaver, W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. New York: Wiley.
Stoffregen, T. A., Yang, C. M., Giveans, M. R., Flanagan, M., & Bardy, B. G. (2009). Movement in the perception of an affordance for wheelchair locomotion. Ecological Psychology, 21, 1-36.
Tu, J. H., Lin, Y. F., & Chin, S. C. (2010). The influence of ball velocity and court illumination on reaction time for tennis volley. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine, 9, 56-61.
Thelen, E., & Ulrich, B. D. (1991). Hidden skills: A dynamical systems analysis of treadmill stepping during the first year. Monographs of Society for Research in Child Development, 56, 1-98.
Tang, M. Y., & Yang, C. M. (2014, August). The method of information acquiring improve judging the contacting directions of table tennis balls. Oral session presented at the 7th Asian-South Pacific Association of Sport Psychology International Congress, Tokyo.
Warren, W. H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Human Perception and Performance, 10, 683-703.
Wagman, J. B., Carello, C., Schmidt, R. C., & Turvey, M. T. (2009). Is perceptual learning unimodal?. Ecological Psychology, 21, 37-67.
Wang, T., Lavis, Y., Hall, G., & Mitchell, C. J. (2012). Location and salience of unique features in human perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 38, 407-418.
Watanabe, T., Náñez, J. E., & Sasaki, Y. (2001). Perceptual learning without perception. Nature, 413, 844-848.
Warren, W. H., & Whang, S. (1987). Visual guidance of walking through apertures: Body-scaled information for affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 371-383.
Zebrowitz, L. A., & Collins, M. A. (1997). Accurate social perception at zero acquaintance: The affordances of a Gibsonian approach. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 204-223.