簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 陳昱君
CHEN, Yuchun
論文名稱: 學齡前特定型語言障礙兒童新詞學習與音韻處理能力之探究
Novel word learning and phonological processing abilities in preschool children with specific language impairment
指導教授: 劉惠美
Liu, Huei-Mei
學位類別: 博士
Doctor
系所名稱: 特殊教育學系
Department of Special Education
論文出版年: 2012
畢業學年度: 100
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 142
中文關鍵詞: 特定型語言障礙快速配對新詞學習音韻工作記憶語音知覺
英文關鍵詞: specific language impairment, fast mapping, novel word learning, phonological working memory, speech perception
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:348下載:74
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 本研究的主要目的是探討特定型語言障礙兒童在不同形式作業下的新詞學習歷程表現,以及新詞學習表現和不同層次的音韻處理能力之間的關連性,進而推論特定型語言障礙兒童在新詞學習困難的來源。同時,基於詞彙能力對整體語言發展的重要性,也探討詞彙能力、新詞學習和相關音韻處理能力對於學前兒童語言理解表現的整體預測力。

    研究對象為41名學前特定型語言障礙兒童,及37名生理年齡對照組的一般兒童。所有受試者皆接受標準化智力測驗、語言相關測驗以及自編的新詞學習作業,包含了「快速配對」和「新詞命名學習」兩種。在音韻處理能力上,則納入「音韻工作記憶」和「語音知覺」兩個層次。

    本研究的主要發現包括:
    一、學前特定型語言障礙兒童在快速配對的表現與一般兒童沒有差異,但在需要建立穩定音韻表徵的新詞命名學習作業形式上的表現較差,且隨著測試次數增加和一般兒童的差異越大。

    二、學前特定型語言障礙兒童的音韻工作記憶(包括非詞複誦和數字廣度作業)較一般對照組差,但在視覺空間工作記憶上的表現則與一般對照組相當。在語音知覺能力上,特定型語言障礙兒童不論是在音段層次的塞音區辨,或是超音段的聲調區辨敏感度都較一般兒童差。

    三、在控制年齡、智力與構音能力的影響之後,音韻處理能力和新詞命名學習的表現相關,但與快速配對的表現之間則沒有關連性。迴歸分析的結果顯示,兒童的非詞複誦能力和聲調區辨敏感度皆對於新詞命名學習有直接的影響,但僅有非詞複誦能力對於口語詞彙理解(PPVT)有獨立的變異解釋量。

    四、最後,本研究的結果發現除了年齡、非語文智力之外,在對於學前兒童的整體語言理解能力的預測上,口語詞彙理解能力、新詞命名學習、非詞複誦正確率和聲調區辨敏感度皆有獨立的變異解釋力。

    總結而言,本研究發現學前特定型語言障礙兒童的新詞學習困難發生在音韻表徵的建立過程,研究結果也支持其困難來源為音韻處理能力上的缺陷。除了既有的口語詞彙理解能力之外,在加入新詞學習與相關音韻處理能力後,可提昇對於學前兒童的整體語言理解能力的預測力,顯示詞彙與相關的音韻處理能力在學前階段對於語言理解發展的重要性。最後,依照本研究的結果提出對於詞彙教學與未來研究上的建議。

    The main purpose of this study was to explore the underlying deficit of novel word learning in preschool children with specific language impairment by comparing their performance in different types of novel word learning tasks and examining the relationship between word learning and phonological processing abilities.

    Forty-one children with SLI and 37 children with normal language development participated in this study. Children’s language, vocabulary and nonverbal IQ were tested using standardized tests and their word learning and related phonological processing abilities were assessed via computerized tasks. The novel word learning task included two formats: ‘fast-mapping’ and ‘novel word repetitive naming tests’, with three types of stimuli either varying in syllable length or semantic head. Two levels of phonological processing abilities were tested, phonological working memory and speech perception (i.e. Chinese lexical tone /yi2-yi3/ and stop consonant /ba-da/ discrimination abilities). In addition to group comparison, this study also examined the correlations between preschool children’s vocabulary, fast-mapping, novel word repetitive naming tests, phonological working memory, speech perception and their predictive powers to their general language comprehension ability.

    Results demonstrated that there were no group differences in fast-mapping abilities. Children with SLI were poorer in naming novel words, and the performance gap between children with SLI and normal language development increased as the number of test phase increases. Comparing to the control group, children with SLI showed deficit in both nonword repetition and speech discrimination tasks. Furthermore, there were significant correlations between children’s phonological processing abilities and novel word naming, but not fast-mapping. The results suggest that the underlying deficit of word learning in children with SLI might be phonologically-based, rather than in the mapping process.

    Finally, children’s novel word naming, phonological working memory , and lexical tone discrimination sensitivity had additional predictive power to their general language comprehension ability after controlling for existing vocabulary ability. The results not only confirmed the importance of early vocabulary and related phonological processing abilities to language development, but also provide empirical data for future intervention plans.

    中文摘要 ………………………………………………………………… i 英文摘要 …………………………………………………………………iii 目錄 ……………………………………………………………………… v 圖目錄 …………………………………………………………………… vi 表目錄 ……………………………………………………………………vii 第一章 緒論 第一節 研究背景……………………………………………………… 1 第二節 研究目的與問題……………………………………………… 4 第二章 文獻探討 第一節 特定型語言障礙兒童之特性………………………………… 6 第二節 學前特定型語言障礙兒童之詞彙表現………………………14 第三節 學前特定型語言障礙兒童之新詞學習………………………19 第四節 特定型語言障礙兒童的音韻處理能力缺陷…………………35 第三章 研究方法 第一節 研究設計與架構………………………………………………47 第二節 研究對象………………………………………………………49 第三節 研究工具與程序………………………………………………53 第四節 資料處理與分析………………………………………………66 第四章 結果與討論 第一節 兩組兒童的新詞學習作業表現………………………………68 第二節 兩組兒童的音韻處理能力……………………………………80 第三節 音韻處理能力和詞彙理解、新詞學習之間的關係…………84 第四節 各變項對整體語言理解能力的預測…………………………95 第五節 綜合討論…………………………………………………… 100 第五章 結論與建議 第一節 結論………………………………………………………… 105 第二節 建議 ……….……………………………………………… 107 參考文獻…………………………………………………………………109 附錄 附錄一 新詞學習作業的新詞刺激與指稱物……………………… 133 附錄二 快速配對作業的表達與理解測試範例…………………… 134 附錄三 快速配對作業理解測試的干擾選項圖片列表…………… 135 附錄四 新詞學習作業的兩個故事腳本與內容理解題目………… 136 附錄五 非詞複誦作業的刺激列表………………………………… 139 附錄六 非詞複誦作業的作業呈現範例…………………………… 140 附錄七 紅點矩陣作業的作業呈現範例…………………………… 141 附錄八 語音區辨作業的作業呈現範例…………………………… 142

    中文部分:
    王南梅、費珮妮、黃恂、陳靜文(1984):三歲至六歲學齡前兒童國語語音發展結構。聽語會刊,1,12-17。
    中國國家語言文字工作委員會(2010):中國語言生活狀況報告。2011年12月31日,取自http://www.china language.gov.cn/14/2011_5_16/1_14_4867_0_1305537251281.html 羊蕙君(2008):記憶支持和誘發表達對學齡前特定型語言障礙兒童快速配對新詞彙之成效探討。國立台北護理學院聽語障礙科學研究所碩士班碩士論文,未出版,台北市。
    李蓁(2005):中文學齡前兒童非詞覆誦測驗與音韻處理能力之探討。國立台灣大學語言學研究所碩士論文,未出版,台北市。
    吳武典、胡心慈、蔡崇建、王振德、林幸台、郭靜姿(2006):托尼非語文智力測驗(再版)。台北:心理。
    林寶貴、林美秀(1993):修訂學前兒童語言障礙評量表指導手冊。台北:國立台灣師範大學特殊教育研究所。
    林寶貴、黃玉枝、黃桂君、宣崇慧(2008):修訂學前兒童語言障礙評量表。台北:國立台灣師範大學特殊教育中心。
    林寶貴、黃玉枝、黃桂君、宣崇慧(2009):修訂學齡兒童語言障礙評量表。台北:國立台灣師範大學特殊教育中心。
    佘永吉(2006):台灣學齡前兒童口語詞彙資料庫之發展。國立成功大學醫學工程研究所博士論文,未出版,台南市。
    竺家寧(2009):漢語詞彙學。台北:五南。
    胡潔芳(2004):音韻覺識與新詞學習:音韻規律學習能力之影響。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(NSC92-2411-H-133-002),未出版。
    張欣戊(1991):學前兒童語言能力的測驗。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(NSC79-0301-H002-067),未出版。
    張顯達、趙文崇(1999):初探特定型語言障礙兒童的語法能力。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(NSC-88-2411-H-002-003),未出版。
    張顯達(2000):三至四歲兒童對國語輔音的聽辨與發音。語言暨語言學,1(2),19-38。
    張顯達、許碧勳(2000):國語輔音聽辨與發音能力之發展研究。中華民國聽力語言學會雜誌,15,1-10
    張顯達、鄒國蘇、趙文崇(2004):特定型語言障礙兒童之語言能力發展─發展轉變與類型之間的探索。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(NSC92-2411-H-002-055-),未出版。
    張顯達(2005):特定型兒童語言障礙兒童的體貌標記處理能力與音韻記憶容量的關係。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(NSC93-2411-H-002-086-),未出版。
    張鑑如、劉惠美、賴文鳳、李俊仁(2011):閱讀研究議題一:影響學前幼兒閱讀與書寫的能力因素分析─學前幼兒閱讀能力指標之建構。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(NSC100-2420-H003-001-MY2),未出版。
    許月琴(2000):台北市五歲孩童特定型語言障礙之調查研究。國立台灣師範大學特殊教育研究所碩士論文,未出版,台北市。
    許馨仁(2002):音韻障礙與句法障礙之關聯性研究。國立台灣大學語言學研究所碩士論文,未出版,台北市。
    陳小娟(1999):三至八歲聽覺障礙兒童語音聽知覺接受力之研究。特殊教育與復健學報,7,51-78。
    陳昱君、劉惠美、李俊仁(2010,3 月):中文3~8歲兒童非詞複誦作業之發展與應用。2010年台灣學障學會年會暨學術研討會海報發表,台中市。
    陳立芸、劉惠美(2010):學齡期特定型語言障礙兒童聽知覺區辨能力初探。特殊教育研究學刊,35, 1, 1-18。
    陳榮華、陳心怡(2000):魏氏幼兒智力量表修訂版。台北:中國行為科學社 曹峰銘、李菁芸、謝怡欣、邱建業(2009):學齡前兒童圔音及聲調知覺與詞彙發展的關係。台灣聽力語言學會雜誌,24,39-58。
    陸莉、劉鴻香(1998):修訂畢保德圖畫詞彙測驗。台北:心理。
    葉麗莉(2004):3;7至4;1歲特定型語言障礙孩童之母子互動的語用形態初探。國立台北護理學院聽語障礙科學研究所碩士班碩士論文,未出版,台北市。
    蔡宛諭(2005):語言障礙兒童敘事能力之研究。輔仁大學語言學研究所碩士論文,未出版,新北市。
    劉惠美、曾進興、曹峰銘(1999):國語語音對比的聲學相關特性。國家科學委員會研究彙刊:人文及社會科學,9(4),726-738。
    劉惠美、曹峰銘(2010):華語嬰幼兒溝通發展量表之編製與應用。中華心理衛生學刊,23(4), 503-534。
    簡秀如(2004):國英雙語幼兒發音與辨音及音韻覺識能力的發展。朝陽科技大學幼兒保育研究所碩士論文,未出版,台中縣。
    鄒啟蓉、張顯達、張鑑如(2006):學齡前特定型語言障礙兒童口語述說能力研究。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(NSC 92-2413-H-154-002),未出版。
    鄭安安(2007):特定型語言障礙兒童的語音知覺特性。國立台灣大學心理研究所碩士論文,未出版,台北市。
    錡寶香(2002a):特定型語言障礙兒童鑑定方式之探討。特殊教育季刊,84,1-8。
    錡寶香(2002b):嬰幼兒溝通能力之發展:家長的長期追蹤記錄。特殊教育學報,16,23-64。
    錡寶香(2007):特定型語言障礙兒童音韻短期記憶能力之初探。特殊教育研究學刊,32,19-45
    錡寶香(2009):特定型語言障礙兒童的詞彙學習與聲韻處理能力之探討。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(NSC97-2410-H152-004),未出版。
    騫心曼 (2006):3-6 歲台灣特定型語言障礙兒童的辭彙習得─以圖片命名作業為例。中原大學心理學研究所碩士論文,未出版,桃園縣。
    英文部分:
    Aitchison, J.(2003). Words in the Mind: An Introduction to the Mental Lexicon.(3rd Ed.). Oxford and New York: Basil Blackwell
    Alloway, T.P. (2007). Automated Working Memory Assessment (AWMA). London: Harcourt Assessment.
    Aram, D. M., Ekelman, B. L., & Nation, J. E. (1984). Preschoolers with language disorders: 10 years later. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 232–244.
    Aram, D. M., & Nation, J. E. (1980). Preschool language disorders and subsequent language and academic difficulties. Journal of Communication Disorders, 13, 159–170.
    Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006a). Short-term and working memory in SLI. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 675-693.
    Archibald, L.M.D. and Gathercole, S.E. (2006b) Visuospatial Immediate Memory in Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 265-277
    Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–90). New York: Academic Press.
    Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.
    Baddeley, A.D. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 417-423.
    Baldwin, D. A., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Word learning: A window on early pragmatic understanding. In E. V. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the Stanford Child Language Research Forum. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.
    Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
    Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: Comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6(2), 183-200.
    Bernhardt, B. M., Kemp, N., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Early word-object associations and later language development. First Language, 27(4), 315-328.
    Bernstein, L. E., & Stark, R. E. (1985). Speech perception development in language-impaired children: A 4-year follow-up study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 21-30.
    Best, C.C., & McRoberts, G.W. (2003). Infant Perception of Non-Native Consonant Contrasts that Adults Assimilate in Different Ways. Language and Speech, 46, 183-216,
    Bishop, D. V. M., North, T. & Donlan, C. (1995). Genetic basis of specific language impairment: evidence from a twin study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 37, 56-71.
    Bishop, D. V.M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 391–403.
    Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: development and disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychology Press.
    Bishop, D. V. M., Price, T. S., Dale P. S. & Plomin, R.(2003). Outcomes of early language delay II: Etiology of transient and persistent language delay. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46, 561-575.
    Bloom, L. (1991). Language development from two to three. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
    Bloom, L. (1993). The transition from infancy to language: Acquiring the power of expression. New York: Cambridge University Press.
    Boersma & Paul (2001). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International 5:9/10, 341-345.
    Bowey, J. A. (2001). Nonword repetition and young children’s receptive vocabulary: A longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 441–469.
    Brown, R. and McNeill, D. (1966). The ‘tip of the tonue’ phenomenon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 5, 325-337.
    Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429-438.
    Carey, S. (1978). The child as word learner. In M. Halle, J. Bresnan, & G. Miller (Eds.) Linguistic theory and psychological reality (pp. 264-293). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 15, 17-29
    Caselli, M. C., Bates, E., Casadio, P., Fenson, J., Fenson, L., Sanderl, L., & Weir, J. (1995). A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development. Cognitive Development, 10, 159-199.
    Catts, H.W., Fey,M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 1142–1157.
    Catts, H.W., Bridges, M., Little, T. & Tomblin, J.B. (2008). Reading achievement growth in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech-Language-Hearing Research, 51, 1569-1579.
    Ceponiene, R., Lepisto, T., Alku, P., Aro, H., & Naatanen, R. (2003). Event-related potential indices of auditory vowel processing in 3-year-old children. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 652-661
    Chen, X., Hao, M., Geva, E., Zhu, H., & Shu, H. (2009). The role of compound awareness in Chinese children’s vocabulary acquisition and character reading. Reading and Writing, 22, 615–631.
    Cheung, H. & Fon, J. (2002). The construction of classifier phrase in child Mandarin. In Y.E. Hsiao(ed.) Proceedings of the First Cognitive Linguistics Conference (pp. 42-57). National Cheng-Chi University, Taipei.
    Cheung, H. (2009). Grammatical characteristics of Mandarin-speaking children with specific language impairment. In S-.P. Law, R.S. Weekes & A. M-Y. Wong (Eds.) Language disorders in speakers of Chinese (pp. 33-52). Bristol, UK: Multilingual matters.
    Choudhury, N., & Benasich, A.A. (2003). A Family Aggregation Study: The Influence of Family History and Other Risk Factors on Language Development. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46, 261-272
    Clahsen, H. (1989). The grammatical characterization of developmental dysphasia. Linguistics, 27, 897-920.
    Coady, J. A., Kluender, K. R., & Evans, J. L. (2005). Categorical perception of speech by children with specific language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 944–959.
    Constable, A., Stackhouse, J. and Wells, B. (1997) Developmental word finding difficulties and phonological processing: the case of the missing handcuffs. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 507–536.
    Conti-Ramsden, G.M., Hesketh, A. (2003). Risk markers for SLI: a study of young language-learning children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38(3), 251-63.
    Corriveau K.H., Pasquni E.S., and Goswami U. (2007). Basic auditory processing skills and specific language impairment: a new look at an old hypothesis. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50, 1–20.
    Demke, T. L., Graham, S. A., & Siakaluk, P. D. (2002). The influence of exposure to phonological neighbours on preschoolers’ novel word production. Journal of Child Language, 29, 379–392.
    Desrochers, A. & Begg, I. (1987). A theoretical account of encoding and retrieval processes in the use of imagery based mnemonic techniques: the special case of the keyword method. In M.A. McDaniel & M. Pressley (Eds.) Imagery and related mnemonic processes (pp. 56-77). Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag.
    Dollaghan, C. A. (1987). Fast mapping in normal and language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 218-222.
    Dollaghan, C., & Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1136-1146.
    Edwards, J., & Lahey, M. (1998). Nonword repetitions of children with specific language impairment: Exploration of some explanations for their inaccuracies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 279-309.
    Edwards, J., Fox, R. A., & Rogers, C. (2002). Final consonant discrimination in children: Effects of phonological disorder, vocabulary size, and phonetic inventory size. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 231-242.
    Eimas, P.D., Siqueland, E.R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303-306
    Ellis Weismer, S. & Hesketh, L. (1993). The influence of prosodic and gestural cues on novel word acquisition by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 36, 1013-1025
    Ellis Weismer, S. & Hesketh, L. (1996). Lexical learning by children with specific language impairment: Effects of linguistic input presented at varying speaking rates. Journal of Speech , Language and Hearing Research, 39, 177-190
    Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J., & Hesketh, L. (1999). An examination of working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1249–1260.
    Ellis Weismer, S., Tomblin, J. B., Zhang,X., Buckwalter, P., Chynoweth, J. G., & Jones, M. (2000). Nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 865-878.
    Ellis Weismer, S. (2005). Speech Perception in Specific Language Impairment. In D. B. Pisoni & R. E. Remez (Eds.), The Handbook of Speech Perception (pp. 567-588). Oxford: Blackwell.
    Erbaugh, M. (1992). The acquisition of Mandarin. In D.I. Slobin (ed.) The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (volume 3, pp. 373-456). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Evans, J. L., Saffran, J. R., & Robe-Torres, K. (2009). Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 52, 321–335.
    Fletcher, P., Leonard, L. B., Stokes, S. F., & Wong, A. M-Y. (2005). The expression of aspect in Cantonese-speaking children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 621-634.
    Frishkoff, G. A., Collins-Thompson, K., Perfetti, C. A., & Callan, J. (2008). Measuring
    incremental changes in word knowledge: Experimental validation and implications for learning and assessment. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 907-25.
    Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213.
    Gathercole, S.E., & Baddeley, A.D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language-disordered children: is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336-360.
    Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887-898.
    Gathercole, S., & Adams, A. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 770–778.
    Gathercole, S. E., & Martin, A. J. (1996). Interactive processes in phonological memory. In S. E. Gathercole (Ed.), Models of short-term memory. Hove: Psychology Press.
    Gathercole, S. E., Hitch, G. J., Service, E., & Martin, A. J. (1997). Short-term memory and new word learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 966-979.
    Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190.
    Gathercole, S. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513-543
    Gleitman, L.R. (1994). Words words words. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 346, 71-77
    Gleitman, L. R., & Gillette, J. (1999). The role of syntax in verb learning. In Fletcher and MacWhinney (eds.) Handbook of child language acquisition. San Diego: Academic Press.
    Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bailey, L. M., & Wenger, N. R. (1992). Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn new nouns. Developmental Psychology, 28, 99-108.
    Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C. B., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1994). Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language, 21, 125-155.
    Gopnik, A., & Choi, S. (1995). Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English- and Korean-speakers: Nouns are not always learned before verbs. In M. Tomasello & W. Merriman (Eds.), Beyond names for things: Young children’s acquisition of verbs. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Graf-Estes K., Evans J.L., & Else-Quest N.M. (2007). Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: a meta-analysis. Journal of speech, language and hearing research, 50, 177-195
    Gray, S., Plante, E., Vance, R., & Henrichsen, M. (1999). The Diagnostic Accuracy of Four Vocabulary Tests Administered to Preschool-Age Children, Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 196-206.
    Gray, S. (2003). Word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: What predicts success? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 56-67.
    Gray, S. (2004). Word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: Predictors and poor learners. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 1117-1132.
    Gray, S. (2005). Word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: Effect of phonological or semantic cues. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 1452-1467
    Gray, S. (2006). The Relationship Between Phonological Memory, Receptive Vocabulary, and Fast Mapping in Young Children With Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 49. 955-969
    Gray, S. and Brinkley, S. (2011). Fast mapping and word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment in a supported learning context: Effect of encoding Cues, phonotactic probability, and object familiarity. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 54. 870-884
    Gupta, P., & Tisdale, J. (2009). Word learning, phonological short-term memory, phonotactic probability and long-term memory: towards an integrated framework. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 3775–3771.
    Hall, D. G., & Graham, S. A. (1999). Lexical form class information guides word-to-object mapping in preschoolers. Child Development, 70, 78-91.
    Hall, D. G., Waxman, S. R., Bredart, S., & Nicolay, A.C. (2003). Preschoolers’ use of form class cues to learn descriptive proper names. Child Development, 74, 1547-1560.
    Hansson, K., Nettelbladt, U., & Leonard, L.B. (2000). Specific language impairment in Swedish: The status of verb morphology and word order. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 848-864.
    Haynes, C. (1982). Vocabulary acquisition problems in language disordered children. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of London.
    Heibeck, T. H., & Markman, E. M. (1987). Word learning in children: An examination of fast mapping. Child Development, 58, 1021-1034.
    Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (1996). The preferential looking paradigm reveals emerging language comprehension. In D. McDaniel, C. McKee, & H. Caims (Eds.), Methods for assessing children’s syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Hollich, G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An Emergentist Coalition Model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65(3).
    Hoover, J. R., Storkel, H. L., & Hogan, T. P. (2010). A cross-sectional comparison of the effects of phonotactic probability and neighborhood density on word learning by preschool children. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 100-116.
    Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lycons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236-248.
    Jackson, T. & Plante, E. (1997). Gyral morphology in the posterior sylvian region in families affected by developmental language disorder. Neuropsychology Review, 6, 81-94.
    Johnson, C. J., Beitchman, J. H., Young, A. R., Escobar, M., Atkinson, L., Wilson, B., et al. (1999). Fourteen-year follow-up of children with and without speech/language impairments: Speech/language stability and outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 744-760.
    Kan, P.F., & Windsor, J. (2010). Word Learning in Children With Primary Language Impairment: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 739-756.
    Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L., III (2007). Expanding retrieval practice promotes short-term retention, but equally spaced retrieval enhances long-term retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 33, 704-719.
    Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). The critical importance of retrieval for learning. Science, 319, 966-968.
    Kiernan, B.,& Gray, S. (1998).Word learning in a supportedlearning context by preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 161-171.
    Kuhl, P K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S. & Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9, F13-F21.
    Kuhl, P.K., Conboy, B.T., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Nelson, T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transaction s of The Royal Society B, 363, 979-1000
    Lahey, M. & Edwards, J. (1999). Naming errors of children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 195-205.
    Leonard, L.B., Camarata, S., Rowan, L.E., & Chapman, K. (1982). The communicative functions of lexical usage by language impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 3, 109-125
    Leonard, L.B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Leonard, L. B. & Deevy, P. (2003). Lexical deficits of children with specific language impairment. In L. Verhoeven & H. van Balkom, (eds.), Classification of developmental Language Disorders: Theoretical Issues and Clinical Implications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Lincoln, A.J., Courchesne, E., Harms, L., & Allen, M. (1995).Sensory Modulation of Auditory Stimuli in Children with Autism and Receptive Developmental Language Disorder: Event-Related Brain Potential Evidence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 521-539
    Mainela-Arnold, E., Evans, J. L., & Coady, J. A. (2010). Beyond capacity limitations: II. Effects of lexical processeson word recall in verbal working memory tasks in children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 53, 1656-1672.
    Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Marinellie, S. A., & Johnson, C. J. (2002). Definitional skill in school-age children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 35, 241-259.
    McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children’s Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 743–751.
    McGregor, K.K. (1997). The nature of word-finding errors of preschoolers with and without word-finding deficits. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 40, 1232-1245.
    McGregor, K.K. & Appel, A. (2002). On the relation between mental representation and naming in a child with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 16, 1-20.
    McGregor, K., Friedman, R. M., Reilly, R. M., & Newman, R. M. (2002). Semantic representations in young children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 332–346.
    McGregor, K., Sheng, L., & Smith, B. (2005). The precocious two-year-old: Status of the lexicon and links to the grammar. Journal of Child Language, 32, 563-585.
    Mervis, C., & Bertrand, J. (1994). Acquisition of the novel name-nameless category (NC3)principle. Child Development, 65, 1646-1662.
    Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89-120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Metsala, J. L. (1999). The development of phonemic awareness in reading disabled children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 149–158.
    Merzenich, M. M., Jenkins, W. M., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S. L., & Tallal, P. (1996). Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training. Science, 271, 77-81.
    Miller, C.A., Kail, R., Leonard, L.B., & Tomblin, J.B. (2001). Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 416-433.
    Mills, D., Prat, C., Zangl, R., Stager, C.L.,Neville, H.J., & Werker, J.F. (2004). Language experience and the organization of brain activity to phonetically similar words: ERP evidence from 14-and 20-month-olds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1452–1464.
    Molfese,D. L. 2000 Predicting dyslexia at 8 years of age using neonatal brain responses. Brain and Language, 72, 238–245.
    Montgomery, J. (2000a). Verbal working memory in sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 293–308.
    Montgomery, J. (2000b). Relation of working memory to off-line and real-time sentence processing in children with Specific Language Impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 117-148.
    Moyle, M. J., Weismer, S. E., Evans, J. L., & Lindstrom, M. J. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late-talking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 508-528.
    Nash, M., & Donaldson, M. L. (2005). Word learning in children with vocabulary deficits. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 439-458.
    Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38, 1-135.
    Nelson, K. (1988). Constraints on word learning? Cognitive Development, 3, 221-246.
    Neville, H.J., Coffey, S.A., Holcomb, P.J., & Tallal, P. (1993). The Neurobiology of Sensory and Language Processing in Language-Impaired Children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 235-253.
    Newman, R., Ratner, N. B., Jusczyk, A. M., Jusczyk, P. W. & Dow, K. A. 2006 Infants’ early ability to segment the conversational speech signal predicts later language development: a retrospective analysis. Developmental Psychology, 42, 643–655.
    NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Pathways to reading: The role of oral language in the transition to reading. Developmental Psychology, 41 (2), 428-442.
    Nippold, M.A. (2007). Later language development: School-age children, adolescents, and young adults (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed
    Nittrouer, S., & Lowenstein, J. H. (2007). Children’s weighting strategies for word-final stop voicing are not explained by auditory sensitivities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 58-73.
    Oetting, J. B., Rice, M. L., & Swank, L. K. (1995). Quick incidental learning (QUIL) of words by school-age children with and without SLI. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 434–445.
    Owens, A.J. & Leonard, L.B. (2006). The Production of Finite and Nonfinite Complement Clauses by Children With Specific Language Impairment and Their Typically Developing Peers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 548-571.
    Owens, R.E. (2007). Language development: An introduction (7th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
    Paul, R. (1993). Patterns of development in late talkers: Preschool years. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 15, 7-14.
    Pease, D., & Berko Gleason, J. (1985). Gaining meaning: Semantic development. In J. Berko Gleason, (Ed.) The development of language. Columbus, OH: Merrill
    Plante, E., Swisher, L., Vance, R., & Rapcsak, S. (1991). MRI findings in boys with specific language impairment. Brain and Language, 41, 52-66.
    Plante, E., Bahl, M., Vance, R., & Gerken, L.A. (2011) Beyond phonotactic frequency: Presentation frequency effects word productions in specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, 91-102.
    Plomin, R., & Dale, P.S. (2000). Genetics and early language development : A UK study of twins. In D.V.M. Bishop & L.B. Leonard. (Eds.) Speech and Language Impairments in children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention and Outcomes. Hove: Psychology Press.
    Polka, L., Colantonio, C., & Sundara, M. (2001). A cross-language comparison of /d/-/ ð / perception: Evidence for a new developmental pattern. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109(5), 2190-2201.
    Quine, W. V. O. (1960). Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Rescorla, L., Roberts, J., & Dahlsgaard, K. (1997). Late talkers at 2: Outcome at age 3. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 556–566.
    Rice, M.L., Buhr, J., & Nemeth, J. (1990). Fast mapping word learning abilities of language delayed preschoolers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 33-42
    Rice, M.L., Buhr. J., & Oetting, J. (1992). Specific-language-impaired children's quick incidental learning of words: The effect of a pause. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1040-1048.
    Rice, M.L., & Bode, J. (1993). GAPS in the verb lexicons of children with specific language impairment. First Language, 13, 113-131.
    Rice, M.L., Oetting, J.B., Marquis, J., Bode, J., & Pae, S. (1994). Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 106-122.
    Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (2001). Rice/Wexler Test of Early Grammatical Impairment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychology Corporation.
    Rice, M. L., Tomblin, J. B., Hoffman, L, Richman, W. A., & Marquis, J. (2004). Grammatical tense deficits in children with SLI and nonspecific language impairment: Relationships with nonverbal IQ over time. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 816-834.
    Rice, M. L. (2009). Language Acquisition Lessons from Children with Specific Language Impairment: Revisiting the Discovery of Latent Structures. In V. C. M. Gathercole (Ed.), Routes to Language: Studies in Honor of Melissa Bowerman (pp. 287-313). New York, London: Taylor & Francis Group.
    Robertson, E.K., Joanisse, A.S.D., & Ng, S. (2009). Categorical speech perception eficits istinguish language and reading impairments in children. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 2190-2201.
    Roid, G. M., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter International Performance Scale–Revised: Examiners manual. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Co.
    Sansavini, A., Guarini, A., Alessandroni, R., Faldella, G., Giovanelli, G., & Salvioli, G. (2006). Early relations between lexical and grammatical development in very immature Italian preterms. Journal of Child Language, 33, 199–216.
    Scarborough, H. (1989). Prediction of reading dysfunction from familial and individual differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 101-108.
    Shu, H., Peng, H., & McBride-Chang, C. (2008). Phonological awareness in young Chinese children. Developmental Science, 11, 171-181.
    The SLI consortium (SLIC). (2002). A genomewide scan identifies two novel loci involved in specific language impairment. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 70, 384-398.
    The SLI consortium (SLIC). (2004). Highly significant linkage to the SLI1 locus in an expanded sample of individuals affected by specific language impairment. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 74, 1225-1238.
    Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word learning tasks. Nature, 388(6640), 381-382.
    Stark, R., & Tallal, P. (1981). Selection of children with specific language deficits. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46, 114-122.
    Stark, R. E., & Heinz, J. M. (1996a). Perception of stop consonants in children with expressive and receptive–expressive language impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 676-686.
    Stark, R. E., & Heinz, J. M. (1996b). Vowel perception in children with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 860-869.
    Stokes, S. and Fletcher, P. (2000). Lexical diversity and productivity in Cantonese- speaking children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 527-541.
    Stokes, S. F. & Fletcher, P. (2003). Aspectual forms in Cantonese-speaking children with specific language impairment. Linguistics, 41(2), 381-406.
    Stokes, S.F., Wong, A.M-Y., Fletcher, P. and Leonard, L.B. (2006). Non-word repetition and sentence repetition as clinical markers of specific language impairment. The case of Cantonese. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 219-236.
    Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934-947.
    Storkel, H.L. (2001). Learning new words: Phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 1321-1338.
    Storkel, H. L. (2003). Learning new words II: phonotactic probability in verb learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1312-1323.
    Storkel, H. L., & Adolf, S. M. (2009). The effect of semantic set size on word learning by preschool children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 289-305.
    Storkel, H. L., & Hoover, J. R. (2010). Word learning by children with phonological delays: differentiating effects of phonotactic probability and neighborhood density. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43, 105-119.
    Stothard, S.E., Snowling, M.J., Bishop, D.V.M., Chipchase, B.B., & Kaplan, C.A. (1998). Language-Impaired Preschoolers: A Follow-Up Into Adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 407–418
    Streeter, L.A. (1976). Language perception of 2-month-old infants shows effects of both innate mechanisms and experience. Nature, 259, 39-41
    Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Genesee, F. (2006). Language-experience facilitates discrimination of /d- ð / in monolingual and bilingual acquisition of English. Cognition, 100, 369-388
    Sussman, J. E. (1993). Perception of formant transition cues to place of articulation in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1286 -1299.
    Swingley, D., & Aslin, R.N. (2007). Lexical competition in young children's word learning. Cognitive Psychology, 54, 99-132.
    Tallal, P. & Piercy, M. (1973a). Developmental aphasia: Impaired rate of non-verbal processing as a function of sensory modality. Neuropsychologia, 11, 389-398.
    Tallal, P. & Piercy, M. (1973b). Deficits of non-verbal auditory perception in children with developmental aphasia. Nature, 241, 468-469.
    Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1975). Developmental aphasia: The perception of brief vowels and extended stop consonants. Neuropsychologia, 13, 69-74.
    Tallal, P., & Stark, R. (1981). Speech acoustic-cue discrimination abilities of normally developing and language-impaired children. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 69, 568-574.
    Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S. S., et al. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 81-84.
    Tardif, T., Gelman, S.A., & Xu, F. (1999). Putting the "noun bias" in context: A comparison of Mandarin and English, Child Development, 70, 620-635.
    Tager-Flusberg, H. & Cooper, J. (1999). Present and future possibilities for defining a phenotype for specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1275-1278.
    Templin, M. (1957). Certain language skill in children: their development and interrelateionships (Monograph Series No. 26). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, The Institute of Child Welfare.
    Thibodeau, L., & Sussman, H. (1979). Performance on a test of categorical perception of speech in normal and communicatively disordered children. Journal of Phonetics, 7, 375-391.
    Tomblin, J. B., Freese, P. R., & Records, N. L. (1992). Diagnosing specific language impairment in adults for the purpose of pedigree analysis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 832–843.
    Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O'Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 40(6), 1245-1260.
    Trauner, D., Wulfeck, B., Tallal, P., & Hesselink, J. (1995). Neurologic and MRI profiles of language impaired children. Technical Report CND-9513, Center for Research in Language, University of California at San Diego.
    Trehub, S.E. (1976). The discrimination of foreign speech contrasts by infants and adults. Child Development, 47, 466-472.
    Tsao, F. M., Liu, H. M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in second year of life: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 75(4), 1067-1084.
    Tsao, F.-M., Liu, H.-M., Kuhl, P. K. (2006). Perception of native and non-native affricate-fricative contrasts: Cross-language tests on adults and infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 2285-2294.
    Vance, M., Rosen, S., & Coleman, M. (2009). Assessing speech perception in young children and relationships with language skills. International Journal of Audiology, 48, 708-717.
    Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (2001). Emergent literacy skills, early instruction, and individual differences as determinants of difficulties in learning to read: The case for early intervention. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 295–321). New York: Guilford Press.
    Vlach, H. A., & Sandhofer, C. M. (2011). Developmental differences in children’s context-dependent word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 394-401.
    Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities andword-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468-479.
    Walley, A.C. (2005). Speech Perception in Childhood. In D. B. Pisoni & R. E. Remez (Eds.), The Handbook of Speech Perception (pp. 449-468). Oxford: Blackwell.
    Watkins, R.V., Rice, M.L., & Moltz, C.C. (1993). Verb use by language-impaired and normally developing children. First Language, 13, 113-131.
    Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49-63.
    Werker, J. F., Fennell, C. T., Corcoran, K. M., & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants’ ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy, 3, 1-30.
    Werker, J. F. & Yeung, H. H. (2005) Infant speech perception bootstraps word learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(11), 519-527.
    Whitehurst, G. J.,&Lonigan, C. J. (2001). Emergent literacy: Development from prereaders to readers. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 11–29). New York: Guilford Press.
    Wilkinson, K. M., Ross, E., & Diamond, A. (2003). Fast mapping of multiple words: Insights into when “the information provided” does and does not equal ‘the information perceived”. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 739-762.
    Windsor, J. & Hwang, M. (1999). Testing the generalized slowing hypothesis in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1205-1218.
    Wong, P., Schwartz, R.G., & Jenkins, J.J. (2003). Perception and Production of Lexical Tones by 3-Year-Old, Mandarin-Speaking Children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 1065-1079.
    Yu, B., Zhang, W., Jing, Q, Peng, R, Zhang, G., & Simon, H. A. (1985). STM capacity for Chinese and English language materials. Memory and Cognition, 13, 202-207
    Zhu, H., & Dodd, B. (2000). The phonological acquisition of Putonghua (Modern Standard Chinese). Journal of Child Language, 27, 3-42
    Ziegler, J. C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., Alario, F. X., & Lorenzi, C. (2005). Deficits in speech perception predict language learning impairment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 14110-14115.

    Ziegler, J.C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., & Lorenzi, C. (2011). Noise on, voicing off: Speech perception deficits in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 362-372.

    下載圖示
    QR CODE