簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 何彥蓉
Ho, Yen-Jung
論文名稱: 以「動物訓練」情境化認知訓練遊戲促進執行功能:抑制與轉換
Using Cognitive Training Games with "Animal Training Context" to Enhance Executive Functions: Inhibition and Shifting
指導教授: 陳志洪
Chen, Zhi-Hong
口試委員: 陳志洪
Chen, Zhi-hong
郭郡羽
Kuo, Chun-Yu
游志弘
Yu, Chih-Hung
口試日期: 2024/07/10
學位類別: 碩士
Master
系所名稱: 資訊教育研究所
Graduate Institute of Information and Computer Education
論文出版年: 2024
畢業學年度: 112
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 76
中文關鍵詞: 執行功能認知技能訓練數位遊戲式學習情境化動機
英文關鍵詞: Executive Functions, Cognitive Skill Training, Digital Game-Based Learning, Contextualization, Motivation
研究方法: 準實驗設計法
DOI URL: http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202401283
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:212下載:0
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報

執行功能是資訊處理過程中高層次的認知能力,透過認知能力來解決問題或是達成目標,與許多日常生活的反應與思考息息相關。執行功能中的抑制和轉換,是二種基本但重要的能力。抑制則負責壓抑不相關的資訊或干擾,幫助我們在各種情況下保持冷靜與理智;轉換則掌管在任務或心理狀態之間的轉換能力,使我們能夠在應對不同情境或環境變化時,靈活調整並轉變其行為和思想。先前研究指出可以透過訓練來提升執行功能能力,像是反覆練習、漸進式挑戰以及根據認知理論設計的訓練,然而訓練常常需要仰賴重複性高的練習,如果可以提高對學習者的參與意願,進行長時間訓練,可能帶來更好的學習效果以及學習遷移。
因此,本研究聚焦於執行功能中的兩項基礎認知能力:抑制與轉換,並根據這兩項認知能力,開發「動物訓練情境」之認知訓練遊戲。因為當人們處於訓練動物或寵物的情境時,所涉及的自我調整要素,可能也適合運用於自我的認知訓練,例如:專注(注意動物反應)、監控(持續關注動物的狀態和行為)、回饋(適時給予鼓勵或獎賞等明確回饋)、調整(根據結果動態調整訓練策略)等。此外動物或寵物具有親近感,「動物訓練」提供有趣的情境可能有助於引發和維持使用動機,希望當使用者訓練動物時,也同時達到自我訓練和改善。
具體而言,本研究發展以「動物訓練」為主題的情境化訓練遊戲,遊戲情境模擬現實生活的訓練情境,例如對著寵物貓狗說出動作指令,如坐下、握手、撿球、睡覺等,以及提供不同的表演情境,像是在海洋公園參與表演。學習者以訓練者角度,依照訓練的規則,在訓練動物的過程需要抑制文字或圖片的干擾,並且依照情境轉換訓練規則和動作。再透過不同的任務關卡,持續挑戰和練習,進而增強上述兩種認知功能(抑制和轉換)。
本研究探討兩個研究問題:(1) 使用「動物訓練」情境化認知訓練遊戲,對於提升學習者執行功能之抑制與轉換能力的影響?(2) 使用「動物訓練」情境化認知訓練遊戲,對於促進學習者參與動機的影響?研究採用組內設計,對象為臺灣北部大專院校學生44人,將受試者隨機分為A、B兩組,A組先使用「動物訓練情境化」數位遊戲20分鐘,再使用「去情境」數位遊戲 (Stroop Task數位版) 20分鐘;B組順序相反,以排除時間順序所造成的潛在影像。兩組在訓練介入的前後測使用執行功能測驗,以了解學生在使用訓練遊戲後,其執行功能的抑制與轉換能力是否有所提升。最後使用學習動機問卷,收集受試者對於使用兩款數位教材的學習動機。
研究結果顯示: (1) 在使用動物訓練遊戲與去情境訓練遊戲訓練20分鐘後,兩組對學習者的執行功能之抑制與轉換能力皆有顯著進步,但兩組之間的進步幅度無顯著差異。這代表兩種數位遊戲皆可以有效提升學習者的抑制與轉換能力;(2) 在學習動機的部分,學習者使用「情境化」訓練遊戲的整體動機、注意力與相關性二個子面向均顯著高於「去情境」訓練遊戲,表示動物訓練的情境訓練遊戲,更能吸引注意力、貼近生活日常,提供更高的參與動機。

Executive function is a high-level cognitive ability involved in information processing, enabling problem-solving and goal achievement. It is closely related to daily life reactions and thoughts. Inhibition and switching are two fundamental yet critical components of executive function. Inhibition helps suppress irrelevant information or distractions, allowing us to remain calm and rational in various situations, while switching governs the ability to transition between tasks or mental states, enabling flexible adjustment of behavior and thoughts in response to changing contexts or environments. Previous studies have indicated that executive function can be improved through training, such as repeated practice, progressive challenges, and training designed based on cognitive theories. However, traditional training often relies on repetitive practice, which, if the engagement and motivation of learners can be enhanced, might lead to better learning outcomes and transfer of learning.
Therefore, this study focuses on the two basic cognitive abilities of executive function: inhibition and switching. It develops a cognitive training game set in the context of "animal training" based on these two cognitive abilities. When people are in situations where they train animals or pets, the elements of self-regulation involved may also be suitable for cognitive training. These elements include focus (paying attention to animal responses), monitoring (continuously observing the animal's state and behavior), feedback (providing timely encouragement or rewards), and adjustment (dynamically adjusting training strategies based on results). Additionally, animals or pets have a sense of closeness; the "animal training" scenario provides an interesting context that may help trigger and maintain motivation. The idea is that while users train animals, they also engage in self-training and improvement.
Specifically, this study develops a situational training game themed around "animal training." The game scenario simulates real-life training situations, such as giving commands to pet cats or dogs like sit, shake hands, fetch, and sleep, as well as different performance scenarios like participating in shows at an aquarium. Learners, in the role of trainers, need to suppress distractions from words or images during the training process and switch between training rules and actions according to the context. Through different task levels, learners continuously challenge and practice, thereby enhancing the aforementioned two cognitive functions (inhibition and switching).
This study explores two research questions: (1) What is the effect of using a situational cognitive training game themed around "animal training" on improving learners' executive function, specifically inhibition and switching abilities? (2) What is the effect of using a situational cognitive training game themed around "animal training" on promoting learners' motivation to participate? The study uses a within-subjects design with 44 college students from northern Taiwan as participants. Participants are randomly divided into two groups, A and B. Group A first uses the "animal training situational" digital game for 20 minutes, then uses the "decontextualized" digital game (Stroop Task digital version) for 20 minutes. Group B follows the opposite order to eliminate potential effects caused by the order of tasks. Both groups take executive function tests before and after the training intervention to determine whether their inhibition and switching abilities improve after using the training games. Finally, a learning motivation questionnaire is used to collect participants' motivation for using the two digital materials.
The results indicate that: (1) After 20 minutes of training with both the animal training game and the decontextualized training game, both groups show significant improvement in their inhibition and switching abilities, with no significant difference in the improvement between the two groups. This suggests that both digital games effectively enhance learners' inhibition and switching abilities; (2) In terms of learning motivation, learners' overall motivation, attention, and relevance are significantly higher for the "situational" training game compared to the "decontextualized" training game. This indicates that the animal training situational training game is more effective in attracting attention and relating to daily life, thus providing higher engagement motivation.

第一章 緒論 1 第一節 研究背景與動機 1 第二節 研究目的 4 第二章 文獻探討 5 第一節 執行功能 5 第二節 數位遊戲式學習 11 第三節 認知訓練遊戲 17 第三章 系統設計 28 第一節 遊戲訓練原理 28 第二節 「寵物訓練」系統設計 29 第三節 「海洋公園」系統設計 33 第四節 「去情境化」系統設計 36 第四章 研究方法 41 第一節 研究設計 41 第二節 研究對象 42 第三節 介入系統 42 第四節 資料收集工具 43 第四節 資料分析 47 第五章 結果與討論 48 第一節 學習成效 48 第二節 學習動機 51 第六章 結論與建議 57 第一節 研究結論 57 第二節 研究貢獻 58 第三節 研究限制 59 參考文獻 61 中文部分 61 英文部分 61 附錄一 ARCS動機問卷 69 附錄二 寵物訓練遊戲階段規則 71 附錄三 寵物訓練遊戲階段規則 73 附錄四 「去情境」訓練遊戲STROOP階段規則 75

林芳如 (2012)。淺談執行功能及其在特殊教育上的應用。國小特殊教育,第五十四期,23-32。
邱姿、葉在庭(2022)。執行功能, 注意力缺失過動症狀與心智理論能力探討—以大學生為對象,特殊教育研究學刊,47 卷 1 期,61-91。
陳映如(2023)。執行功能訓練融入數學課程對國小學童執行功能, 數學創造力及數學學習表現之效果研究。臺灣師範大學教育心理與輔導學系碩士在職專班學位論文, 2023, 1-97。
陳學志(2022):《新住民子女認知與情緒創造力優勢之行為、神經科學及教學研究:文化暨冷、熱執行功能之調節中介效果第一年期中報告》。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(編號:110-2511-H-003-015-MY3),未出版。
游孟諺(2020)。執行功能訓練融入國語課程對國小學童執行功能, 創造性思考及國語學習表現之效果研究。臺灣師範大學創造力發展碩士在職專班學位論文, 2020, 1-122。

Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., . . . Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in eastern and western countries: a meta-analytic review. Psychological bulletin, 136(2), 151.
Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., Janowich, J., ... & Gazzaley, A. (2013). Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature, 501(7465), 97-101.
Ansari, M., Abdolrahmani, M., & Rezapour, T. (2024). The Effectiveness of the Self-Control Training Video Game Slankers: A Pilot Study. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1-13.
Arieh, Y., & Algom, D. (2002). Processing picture--word stimuli: The contingent nature of picture and of word superiority. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(1), 221.
Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. Journal of personality and social psychology, 41(3), 586.
Benassi, M., Paolillo, D., Spinoso, M., Giovagnoli, S., Mazzoni, N., Formica, L., ... & Prandi, C. (2024, January). Train your attention and executive functions with Eye-Riders! A videogame for improving cognitive abilities in neurodiverse children. In 2024 IEEE 21st Consumer Communications & Networking Conference (CCNC) (pp. 1-6). IEEE.
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child development, 81(6), 1641-1660.
Blair, C. (2016). Executive function and early childhood education. Current opinion in behavioral sciences, 10, 102-107.
Carlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less is more: Executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychological science, 16(8), 609-616.
Cicerone, K., Levin, H., Malec, J., Stuss, D., & Whyte, J. (2006). Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for executive function: moving from bench to bedside in patients with traumatic brain injury. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 18(7), 1212-1222.
De Freitas, S. (2018). Are games effective learning tools? A review of educational games. Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 21(2), 74-84.
Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Current directions in psychological science, 21(5), 335-341.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, 64, 135.
Diamond, L. (2015). Facing up to the democratic recession. Journal of Democracy, 26(1), 141-155.
Dörrenbächer, S., Müller, P. M., Tröger, J., & Kray, J. (2014). Dissociable effects of game elements on motivation and cognition in a task-switching training in middle childhood. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 110859.
Erhel, S., & Jamet, E. (2013). Digital game-based learning: Impact of instructions and feedback on motivation and learning effectiveness. Computers & education, 67, 156-167.
Gee, J. P. (2005). Learning by design: Good video games as learning machines. E-learning and Digital Media, 2(1), 5-16.
Hickmott, D., Smith, S. P., Bille, R., Burd, E., Stephens, L., & Southgate, E. (2016). Building apostrophe power: lessons learnt for serious games development. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the Australasian Computer Science Week Multiconference.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Zosh, J. M., Golinkoff, R. M., Gray, J. H., Robb, M. B., & Kaufman, J. (2015). Putting education in “educational” apps: Lessons from the science of learning. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 16(1), 3-34.
Homer, B. D., Ober, T. M., & Flynn, R. M. (2018). Children and adolescents' development of executive functions in digital contexts. In Proceedings of the Technology, Mind, and Society (pp. 1-3).
Homer, B. D., Plass, J. L., Rose, M. C., MacNamara, A. P., Pawar, S., & Ober, T. M. (2019). Activating adolescents’“hot” executive functions in a digital game to train cognitive skills: The effects of age and prior abilities. Cognitive Development, 49, 20-32.
Hughes, C. (2011). Changes and challenges in 20 years of research into the development of executive functions. Infant and child Development, 20(3), 251-271.
Hwang, G.-J., Sung, H.-Y., Hung, C.-M., Huang, I., & Tsai, C.-C. (2012). Development of a personalized educational computer game based on students’ learning styles. Educational Technology Research and Development, 60(4), 623-638.
Keller, J. M. (1983). Motivational design of instruction. Instructional design theories and models: An overview of their current status, 1(1983), 383-434.
Keller, J. M. (1987). Development and use of the ARCS model of instructional design. Journal of instructional development, 10(3), 2-10.
Keller, J. M. (2008). First principles of motivation to learn and e3‐learning. Distance education, 29(2), 175-185.
Kim, B., Park, H., & Baek, Y. (2009). Not just fun, but serious strategies: Using meta-cognitive strategies in game-based learning. Computers & Education, 52(4), 800-810.
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge university press.
Lenhart, A., Kahne, J., Middaugh, E., Macgill, A. R., Evans, C., & Vitak, J. (2008). Teens, Video Games, and Civics: Teens' Gaming Experiences Are Diverse and Include Significant Social Interaction and Civic Engagement. Pew internet & American life project.
Liu, T.-Y., & Chu, Y.-L. (2010). Using ubiquitous games in an English listening and speaking course: Impact on learning outcomes and motivation. Computers & Education, 55(2), 630-643.
Lumsden, J., Edwards, E. A., Lawrence, N. S., Coyle, D., & Munafò, M. R. (2016). Gamification of cognitive assessment and cognitive training: a systematic review of applications and efficacy. JMIR serious games, 4(2), e5888.
Martens, R., Gulikers, J., & Bastiaens, T. (2004). The impact of intrinsic motivation on e‐learning in authentic computer tasks. Journal of computer assisted learning, 20(5), 368-376.
Mayer, R. E. (2014). Introduction to multimedia learning.
Mayer, R. E., & Johnson, C. I. (2010). Adding instructional features that promote learning in a game-like environment. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 42(3), 241-265.
Mayer, R. E., Parong, J., & Bainbridge, K. (2019). Young adults learning executive function skills by playing focused video games. Cognitive Development, 49, 43-50.
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental psychology, 49(2), 270.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive psychology, 41(1), 49-100.
Monsell, S. (2021). Control of mental processes. In Unsolved mysteries of the mind (pp. 93-148): Psychology Press.
Morosanova, V. I., Bondarenko, I. N., Fomina, T. G., & Velichkovsky, B. B. (2021). Executive functions and сonscious self-regulation as predictors of native language learning success in Russian middle school children.
Morris, N., & Jones, D. M. (1990). Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. British journal of psychology, 81(2), 111-121.
Nussbaum, M., & de Sousa Beserra, V. (2014). Educational videogame design. Paper presented at the 2014 IEEE 14th International Conference on Advanced Learning Technologies.
Papastergiou, M. (2009). Digital game-based learning in high school computer science education: Impact on educational effectiveness and student motivation. Computers & Education, 52(1), 1-12.
Parong, J., Mayer, R. E., Fiorella, L., MacNamara, A., Homer, B. D., & Plass, J. L. (2017). Learning executive function skills by playing focused video games. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 51, 141-151.
Parong, J., Pollard, K. A., Files, B. T., Oiknine, A. H., Sinatra, A. M., Moss, J. D., . . . Khooshabeh, P. (2020). The mediating role of presence differs across types of spatial learning in immersive technologies. Computers in Human Behavior, 107, 106290.
Plass, J. L., Heidig, S., Hayward, E. O., Homer, B. D., & Um, E. (2014). Emotional design in multimedia learning: Effects of shape and color on affect and learning. Learning and Instruction, 29, 128-140.
Plass, J. L., Homer, B. D., & Kinzer, C. K. (2015). Foundations of game-based learning. Educational psychologist, 50(4), 258-283.
Plass, J. L., Homer, B. D., Pawar, S., & Tam, F. (2018). Connecting theory and design through research: Cognitive skills training games. In Serious Games: 4th Joint International Conference, JCSG 2018, Darmstadt, Germany, November 7-8, 2018, Proceedings 4 (pp. 145-158). Springer International Publishing.
Prensky, M. (2003). Digital game-based learning. Computers in entertainment (CIE), 1(1), 21-21.
Prins, P. J., Brink, E. T., Dovis, S., Ponsioen, A., Geurts, H. M., De Vries, M., & Van Der Oord, S. (2013). “Braingame Brian”: toward an executive function training program with game elements for children with ADHD and cognitive control problems. GAMES FOR HEALTH: Research, Development, and Clinical Applications, 2(1), 44-49.
Prins, P. J., Dovis, S., Ponsioen, A., Ten Brink, E., & Van Der Oord, S. (2011). Does computerized working memory training with game elements enhance motivation and training efficacy in children with ADHD? Cyberpsychology, behavior, and social networking, 14(3), 115-122.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American psychologist, 55(1), 68.
Sauvé, L., Renaud, L., Kaufman, D., & Marquis, J. S. (2007). Distinguishing between games and simulations: A systematic review. Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 10(3), 247-256.
Shaffer, D. W. (2006). Epistemic frames for epistemic games. Computers & Education, 46(3), 223-234.
Sung, Y. T., & Wu, J. S. (2018). The visual analogue scale for rating, ranking and paired-comparison (VAS-RRP): a new technique for psychological measurement. Behavior research methods, 50, 1694-1715.
Vermeir, J. F., White, M. J., Johnson, D., Crombez, G., & Van Ryckeghem, D. M. (2020). The effects of gamification on computerized cognitive training: systematic review and meta-analysis. JMIR serious games, 8(3), e18644.
Wang, C., Jaeggi, S. M., Yang, L., Zhang, T., He, X., Buschkuehl, M., & Zhang, Q. (2019). Narrowing the achievement gap in low-achieving children by targeted executive function training. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 63, 87-95.
Wong, Y. K., Wu, C. F., & Tu, Y. H. (2022). Effectiveness of a serious game design and game mechanic factors for attention and executive function improvement in the elderly: a pretest-posttest study. Applied Sciences, 12(14), 6923.
Wouters, P., Van Nimwegen, C., Van Oostendorp, H., & Van Der Spek, E. D. (2013). A meta-analysis of the cognitive and motivational effects of serious games. Journal of educational psychology, 105(2), 249.
Yee, N. (2006). Motivations for play in online games. CyberPsychology & behavior, 9(6), 772-775.
Zin, N. A. M., Jaafar, A., & Yue, W. S. (2009). Digital game-based learning (DGBL) model and development methodology for teaching history. WSEAS transactions on computers, 8(2), 322-333.

無法下載圖示 電子全文延後公開
2026/07/31
QR CODE