簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 郭芷瑄
Kuo, Chih-Hsuan
論文名稱: 探討先備知識、字幕與標籤在動畫輔助學習對冗餘效應之影響
Exploring the Impacts of Subtitles, Labels and Prior Knowledge on Redundancy Effect in Animation Assisted Learning
指導教授: 劉子鍵
Liu, Tzu-Chien
口試委員: 陳學志 吳慧敏 劉子鍵
Liu, Tzu-Chien
口試日期: 2022/01/18
學位類別: 碩士
Master
系所名稱: 教育心理與輔導學系
Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling
論文出版年: 2022
畢業學年度: 110
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 34
中文關鍵詞: 認知負荷冗餘效應字幕指示性標籤先備知識程度
英文關鍵詞: cognitive load, redundancy effect, subtitles, labels, prior knowledge
研究方法: 實驗設計法
DOI URL: http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU202200172
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:137下載:0
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 多媒體教材盛行於現今教學場域,尤其以動態影像為教材者為盛。多媒體教材中經常具備三種表徵:語音、圖像與文字。有鑑於文字與旁白經常互為冗餘,過往研究多建議消弭文字表徵以降低學習者的認知負荷,避免冗餘效應產生。文字表徵的形式多元,不同形式的文字表徵可能會影響冗餘效應的發生與否;此外,由於動態影像帶來的語音瞬逝效應使學習者不易整合教材內容,添加文字表徵可能可降低瞬逝效應帶來的負面影響。是以,本研究的目的之一即在探討動畫教材中常見的兩種文字表徵形式(字幕以及指示性標籤)在搭配語音旁白同時出現時對冗餘效應是否會產生不同的影響。另外,學習者的先備知識程度亦是影響教材設計效果的重要變項,為更深入了解冗餘效應發生之條件,本研究將探討不同文字表徵形式(有無字幕、有無指示性標籤)對於不同先備知識程度(高、低)之學習者的影響。
    本研究使用搭配旁白之動畫教材,主要操弄的變項為字幕的有無和指示性標籤的有無。206名高中一年級學生被隨機分派至字幕與指示性標籤不同組合的四個組別:兼具字幕與指示性標籤組、僅有字幕組、僅有指示性標籤組、無字幕與指示性標籤組,並測量其學習成就表現與認知負荷。在資料分析階段,本研究依照先備知識測驗表現進一步將參與者區分為高先備知識者與低先備知識者,並進行三因子變異數分析。分析結果指出,字幕、指示性標籤與先備知識程度在學習表現上具有顯著的三因子交互作用,且部分支持本研究假設:無論教材中有無指示性標籤,添加字幕皆未降低高先備知識者的學習表現,顯示高先備者並未受到冗餘效應的影響;而對於低先備知識者而言,兼具字幕、指示性標籤教材組別的學習者表現顯著低於僅含有任一種視覺文字表徵(字幕或是指示性標籤)的組別,顯示冗餘效應僅發生在複雜程度較高的教材中。根據上述結果,本研究建議教學者應考量文字表徵不同格式之組合與學習者先備知識程度作為動態多媒體教材設計之指引。

    Multimedia teaching materials are prevalent in today's educational settings, especially in the form of dynamic images. There are three common representations in multimedia materials: narration, image and text. Since text and narration are often redundant with each other, previous studies have often suggested eliminating text representations to reduce the cognitive load of learners and avoid redundancy effect. However, there are multiple forms of text representations, and different forms of text representations may affect the occurrence of redundancy effects. Furthermore, due to transient effect of narration caused by dynamic images, it is difficult for learners to integrate the content of materials, thus adding text representations may reduce the negative impact of the transient effect. In conclusion, one of the purposes of this study is to explore whether subtitles and labels, two common types of text representation in animation, have different effects on the redundancy effect when they are accompanied by narration. In addition, this study considers learners’ prior knowledge as a key variable that affects the effectiveness of teaching materials. To understand the conditions for the occurrence of redundancy effect, the current study explores the effects of different text representations (with or without subtitles, and with or without labels) on learners with different levels (high or low) of prior knowledge.
    This study uses animation with narration as the teaching material, and the main variables are the presence or absence of subtitles and labels. 206 first-year high school students were randomly assigned to four groups with different combinations of subtitles and labels: subtitles and labels, subtitles only, labels only, and no visual text. Then the participants’ learning performance and cognitive load are measured. In the data analysis phase, this study further categorized the participants into those with high prior knowledge and those with low prior knowledge based on their performances in the prior knowledge test, and conducted a three‑way analysis of variance. The results indicate that there was a significant interaction between subtitles, labels, and prior knowledge in terms of learning performance. This partially supports the hypothesis that the addition of subtitles would not negatively influence the performance of high prior knowledge learners regardless of the presence or absence of labels, in other words, high prior knowledge learners are not affected by redundancy effects. As for learners with low prior knowledge, redundancy effects occurred only in the group with both subtitles and labels (i.e. materials with higher level of complexity), as low prior knowledge learners in this group performed significantly worse than those in the groups with only one text representation (subtitles or labels). Based on the findings above, this study suggests that instructors should cautiously choose the forms of text representation and consider learners’ prior knowledge as guidelines for the design of dynamic multimedia materials.

    謝詞 i 中文摘要 ii 英文摘要 iv 目次 vii 表次 viii 圖次 ix 第一章 緒論 1 第二章 文獻探討 4 第一節 認知負荷理論 4 第二節 冗餘效應、字幕與其實證研究 5 第三節 文字標籤與多媒體學習 8 第四節 先備知識與多媒體學習 9 第三章 研究假設 11 第四章 研究方法 14 第一節 實驗設計與研究對象 14 第二節 實驗材料 14 第三節 研究工具 16 第四節 實驗流程 18 第五章 結果與討論 20 第六章 結論 28 第一節 研究貢獻 28 第二節 研究限制與未來建議 29 第三節 教學實務建議 30 參考文獻 31

    Adesope, O. O., & Nesbit, J. C. (2012). Verbal redundancy in multimedia learning environments: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 250–263. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026147
    Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60422-3
    Bobis, J., Sweller, J., & Cooper, M. (1993). Cognitive load effects in a primary-school geometry task. Learning and Instruction, 3(1), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(09)80002-9
    Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 8, 293–332. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci0804_2
    de Koning, B. B., van Hooijdonk, C. M., & Lagerwerf, L. (2017). Verbal redundancy in a procedural animation: On-screen labels improve retention but not behavioral performance. Computers & Education, 107, 45–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2016.12.013
    Fenesi, B., & Kim, J. A. (2014). Learners misperceive the benefits of redundant text in multimedia learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 710. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00710
    Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1998). Levels of expertise and instructional design. Human Factors, 40, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872098779480587
    Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1999). Managing split‐attention and redundancy in multimedia instruction. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 351–371. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199908)13:4<351::AID-ACP589>3.0.CO;2-6
    Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38, 23–31. https://doi.org/10.4018/978-1-60566-048-6.ch003
    Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2000). Incorporating learner experience into the design of multimedia instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 126–136. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.126
    Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2004). When redundant on-screen text in multimedia technical instruction can interfere with learning. Human Factors, 46, 567–581. https://doi.org/10.1518/hfes.46.3.567.50405
    Kutbay, E., & Akpinar, Y. (2020). Investigating Modality, Redundancy and Signaling Principles with Abstract and Concrete Representation. International Journal of Education in Mathematics, Science and Technology, 8, 131–145. https://doi.org/10.46328/ijemst.v8i2.710
    Lee, C. H., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Effectiveness of on-screen pinyin in learning Chinese: An expertise reversal for multimedia redundancy effect. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 11–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.05.005
    Liu, T. C., Lin, Y. C., Gao, Y., Yeh, S. C., & Kalyuga, S. (2015). Does the redundancy effect exist in electronic slideshow assisted lecturing? Computers & Education, 88, 303–314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2015.04.014
    Liu, T. C., Lin, Y. C., & Kalyuga, S. (2021). Strategies for facilitating processing of transient information in instructional videos by using learner control mechanisms [Manuscript submitted for publication]. Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling, National Taiwan Normal University.
    Mayer, R. E., Fiorella, L., & Stull, A. (2020). Five ways to increase the effectiveness of instructional video. Educational Technology Research and Development, 68, 837–852. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-020-09749-6
    Mayer, R. E., Heiser, J., & Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: When presenting more material results in less understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 187–198. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.187
    Mayer, R. E., & Johnson, C. I. (2008). Revising the redundancy principle in multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 380–386. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.380
    Mayer, R., & Mayer, R. E. (Eds.). (2005). The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816819.002
    Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043158
    Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. E. (2002). Verbal redundancy in multimedia learning: When reading helps listening. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 156–163. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.1.156
    Ozcelik, E., Arslan-Ari, I., & Cagiltay, K. (2010). Why does signaling enhance multimedia learning? Evidence from eye movements. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 110–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2009.09.001
    Ozdemir, M., Izmirli, S., & Sahin-Izmirli, O. (2016). The effects of captioning videos on academic achievement and motivation: reconsideration of redundancy principle in instructional videos. Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 19(4), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.18844/wjet.v10i1.3186
    Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–133. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.122
    Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38, 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3801_1
    Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. Springer Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8126-4_18
    Udo, J. P., & Fels, D. I. (2010). The rogue poster-children of universal design: Closed captioning and audio description. Journal of Engineering Design, 21, 207–221. https://doi.org/10.1080/09544820903310691
    Van der Zee, T., Admiraal, W., Paas, F., Saab, N., & Giesbers, B. (2017). Effects of subtitles, complexity, and language proficiency on learning from online education videos. Journal of Media Psychology, 29, 18–30. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000208
    Yue, C. L., Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A. (2013) Reducing verbal redundancy in multimedia learning: An undesired desirable difficulty? Journal of Educational Psychology 105, 266–277. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031971

    下載圖示
    QR CODE